Independent Processing of Stimulus-Stimulus and Stimulus-Response Conflicts
暂无分享,去创建一个
Weizhi Nan | Qi Li | Qi Li | Xun Liu | Xun Liu | Weizhi Nan | Kai Wang | Kai Wang
[1] Kai Wang,et al. Temporal and spectral profiles of stimulus–stimulus and stimulus–response conflict processing , 2014, NeuroImage.
[2] D. Navon. Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception , 1977, Cognitive Psychology.
[3] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[4] Colin M. Macleod. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.
[5] S. Buetti,et al. Approach and avoidance movements are unaffected by cognitive conflict: A comparison of the Simon effect and stimulus–response compatibility , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[6] E. Hazeltine,et al. Domain-specific conflict adaptation without feature repetitions , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[7] G. Humphreys,et al. Analyzing the generality of conflict adaptation effects. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[8] Jin Fan,et al. Cognitive and Brain Consequences of Conflict , 2003, NeuroImage.
[9] Tobias Egner,et al. Separate conflict-specific cognitive control mechanisms in the human brain , 2007, NeuroImage.
[10] J. Lupiáñez,et al. Dissociating proportion congruent and conflict adaptation effects in a Simon-Stroop procedure. , 2013, Acta psychologica.
[11] J. R. Simon. The Effects of an Irrelevant Directional CUE on Human Information Processing , 1990 .
[12] N. Cohen,et al. The relative involvement of anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex in attentional control depends on nature of conflict. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[13] T. Egner,et al. Using neural pattern classifiers to quantify the modularity of conflict-control mechanisms in the human brain. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.
[14] Xun Liu,et al. Common and distinct neural substrates of attentional control in an integrated Simon and spatial Stroop task as assessed by event-related fMRI , 2004, NeuroImage.
[15] J. Ridley. Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .
[16] J R Simon,et al. Processing auditory information: interference from an irrelevant cue. , 1969, The Journal of applied psychology.
[17] Jeounghoon Kim,et al. Multiple cognitive control mechanisms associated with the nature of conflict , 2010, Neuroscience Letters.
[18] Jeounghoon Kim,et al. Conflict adjustment through domain-specific multiple cognitive control mechanisms , 2012, Brain Research.
[19] Jin Fan,et al. Human Attentional Networks: A Connectionist Model , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[20] T. Egner. Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[21] Jin Fan,et al. Dimensional overlap accounts for independence and integration of stimulus—response compatibility effects , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[22] J. Requin,et al. The Effects of Irrelevant Stimuli: 1. The Time Course of Stimulus-Stimulus and Stimulus-Response Consistency Effects With Stroop-Like Stimuli, Simon-Like Tasks, and Their Factorial Combinations , 1999 .
[23] Saul Sternberg,et al. The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .
[24] Robert W. Proctor,et al. Multidimensional vector model of stimulus-response compatibility. , 2012, Psychological review.
[25] Manfred Herrmann,et al. Spatio-temporal brain dynamics in a combined stimulus–stimulus and stimulus–response conflict task , 2011, NeuroImage.
[26] J. Kroger,et al. A functional dissociation of conflict processing within anterior cingulate cortex , 2008, Human brain mapping.
[27] Ty W. Boyer,et al. Stroop interference in a delayed match-to-sample task: evidence for semantic competition , 2013, Front. Psychol..
[28] E. Lauber,et al. Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[29] G. Humphreys,et al. Sustained vs. transient cognitive control: Evidence of a behavioral dissociation , 2010, Cognition.
[30] L. Ferrand,et al. Single-letter coloring and spatial cuing do not eliminate or reduce a semantic contribution to the Stroop effect , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[31] D. Stuss,et al. Stroop performance in focal lesion patients: dissociation of processes and frontal lobe lesion location , 2001, Neuropsychologia.
[32] A. Osman,et al. Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.
[33] Lauren M. Bylsma,et al. The conflict adaptation effect: It’s not just priming , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[34] A. Mecklinger,et al. From orthography to meaning: an electrophysiological investigation of the role of phonology in accessing meaning of Chinese single-character words , 2010, Neuroscience.
[35] J R Simon,et al. Effect of conflicting cues on information processing: the 'Stroop effect' vs. the 'Simon effect'. , 1990, Acta psychologica.
[36] S Kornblum,et al. The way irrelevant dimensions are processed depends on what they overlap with: The case of Stroop- and Simon-like stimuli , 1994, Psychological research.
[37] G. D. Logan. Task Switching , 2022 .
[38] Robert W. Proctor,et al. Stimulus-Response Compatibility: An Integrated Perspective , 1990 .
[39] Chun R. Luo,et al. Semantic Competition as The Basis of Stroop Interference: Evidence From Color-Word Matching Tasks , 1999 .