Independent Processing of Stimulus-Stimulus and Stimulus-Response Conflicts

The dimensional overlap (DO) model proposes distinct mechanisms for stimulus-stimulus (S-S) and stimulus-response (S-R) conflict effects. Many studies have examined the independence of S-S and S-R conflict effects in the color-word Stroop and Simon tasks. However, confounds exist between the distinction of DO (i.e., S-S dimensional overlap compared with S-R dimensional overlap) and the distinction of stimulus attributes (e.g., color compared with spatial location; semantic compared with nonsemantic information), which may hinder interpretation of the independence of S-S and S-R conflicts. A spatial Stroop (word) task and a spatial Stroop (arrow) task were combined with a Simon task in Experiments 1 and 2, respectively to eliminate these confounds of stimulus attributes. The results showed that S-S and S-R conflicts affected performance additively. There was no significant correlation across participants. These findings lend further support to independent processing of S-S and S-R conflicts as it is outlined in the taxonomy of DO.

[1]  Kai Wang,et al.  Temporal and spectral profiles of stimulus–stimulus and stimulus–response conflict processing , 2014, NeuroImage.

[2]  D. Navon Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception , 1977, Cognitive Psychology.

[3]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[4]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[5]  S. Buetti,et al.  Approach and avoidance movements are unaffected by cognitive conflict: A comparison of the Simon effect and stimulus–response compatibility , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[6]  E. Hazeltine,et al.  Domain-specific conflict adaptation without feature repetitions , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[7]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Analyzing the generality of conflict adaptation effects. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  Jin Fan,et al.  Cognitive and Brain Consequences of Conflict , 2003, NeuroImage.

[9]  Tobias Egner,et al.  Separate conflict-specific cognitive control mechanisms in the human brain , 2007, NeuroImage.

[10]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Dissociating proportion congruent and conflict adaptation effects in a Simon-Stroop procedure. , 2013, Acta psychologica.

[11]  J. R. Simon The Effects of an Irrelevant Directional CUE on Human Information Processing , 1990 .

[12]  N. Cohen,et al.  The relative involvement of anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex in attentional control depends on nature of conflict. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[13]  T. Egner,et al.  Using neural pattern classifiers to quantify the modularity of conflict-control mechanisms in the human brain. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  Xun Liu,et al.  Common and distinct neural substrates of attentional control in an integrated Simon and spatial Stroop task as assessed by event-related fMRI , 2004, NeuroImage.

[15]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[16]  J R Simon,et al.  Processing auditory information: interference from an irrelevant cue. , 1969, The Journal of applied psychology.

[17]  Jeounghoon Kim,et al.  Multiple cognitive control mechanisms associated with the nature of conflict , 2010, Neuroscience Letters.

[18]  Jeounghoon Kim,et al.  Conflict adjustment through domain-specific multiple cognitive control mechanisms , 2012, Brain Research.

[19]  Jin Fan,et al.  Human Attentional Networks: A Connectionist Model , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  T. Egner Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  Jin Fan,et al.  Dimensional overlap accounts for independence and integration of stimulus—response compatibility effects , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[22]  J. Requin,et al.  The Effects of Irrelevant Stimuli: 1. The Time Course of Stimulus-Stimulus and Stimulus-Response Consistency Effects With Stroop-Like Stimuli, Simon-Like Tasks, and Their Factorial Combinations , 1999 .

[23]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .

[24]  Robert W. Proctor,et al.  Multidimensional vector model of stimulus-response compatibility. , 2012, Psychological review.

[25]  Manfred Herrmann,et al.  Spatio-temporal brain dynamics in a combined stimulus–stimulus and stimulus–response conflict task , 2011, NeuroImage.

[26]  J. Kroger,et al.  A functional dissociation of conflict processing within anterior cingulate cortex , 2008, Human brain mapping.

[27]  Ty W. Boyer,et al.  Stroop interference in a delayed match-to-sample task: evidence for semantic competition , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[28]  E. Lauber,et al.  Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Sustained vs. transient cognitive control: Evidence of a behavioral dissociation , 2010, Cognition.

[30]  L. Ferrand,et al.  Single-letter coloring and spatial cuing do not eliminate or reduce a semantic contribution to the Stroop effect , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[31]  D. Stuss,et al.  Stroop performance in focal lesion patients: dissociation of processes and frontal lobe lesion location , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[33]  Lauren M. Bylsma,et al.  The conflict adaptation effect: It’s not just priming , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[34]  A. Mecklinger,et al.  From orthography to meaning: an electrophysiological investigation of the role of phonology in accessing meaning of Chinese single-character words , 2010, Neuroscience.

[35]  J R Simon,et al.  Effect of conflicting cues on information processing: the 'Stroop effect' vs. the 'Simon effect'. , 1990, Acta psychologica.

[36]  S Kornblum,et al.  The way irrelevant dimensions are processed depends on what they overlap with: The case of Stroop- and Simon-like stimuli , 1994, Psychological research.

[37]  G. D. Logan Task Switching , 2022 .

[38]  Robert W. Proctor,et al.  Stimulus-Response Compatibility: An Integrated Perspective , 1990 .

[39]  Chun R. Luo,et al.  Semantic Competition as The Basis of Stroop Interference: Evidence From Color-Word Matching Tasks , 1999 .