Intentions in the Brain
暂无分享,去创建一个
Cristina Becchio | Andrea Cavallo | Caterina Ansuini | Cesare Bertone | C. Becchio | A. Cavallo | Cesare Bertone | C. Ansuini
[1] K. Verfaillie,et al. Time Will Show: Real Time Predictions during Interpersonal Action Perception , 2013, PloS one.
[2] U. Castiello,et al. The case of Dr. Jekyll and Mr. Hyde: A kinematic study on social intention , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.
[3] U. Castiello,et al. Both your intention and mine are reflected in the kinematics of my reach-to-grasp movement , 2008, Cognition.
[4] N. Holmes,et al. To eat or not to eat? Kinematics and muscle activity of reach-to-grasp movements are influenced by the action goal, but observers do not detect these differences , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.
[5] J. Searle. Intentionality: An Essay in the Philosophy of Mind , 1983 .
[6] R. Lemon,et al. What We Know Currently about Mirror Neurons , 2013, Current Biology.
[7] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Beyond grasping: Representation of action in human anterior intraparietal sulcus , 2007, NeuroImage.
[8] D. Pandya,et al. Parietal, temporal, and occipita projections to cortex of the superior temporal sulcus in the rhesus monkey: A retrograde tracer study , 1994, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[9] B. Abernethy,et al. Probing expert anticipation with the temporal occlusion paradigm: experimental investigations of some methodological issues. , 2005, Motor control.
[10] Umberto Castiello,et al. Social grasping: From mirroring to mentalizing , 2012, NeuroImage.
[11] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Parietal Lobe: From Action Organization to Intention Understanding , 2005, Science.
[12] Karl J. Friston,et al. Predictive coding: an account of the mirror neuron system , 2007, Cognitive Processing.
[13] Vic Braden,et al. Recognising the Style of Spatially Exaggerated Tennis Serves , 2001, Perception.
[14] M. Jeannerod,et al. Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. , 1987, Canadian journal of psychology.
[15] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Understanding Actions of Others: The Electrodynamics of the Left and Right Hemispheres. A High-Density EEG Neuroimaging Study , 2010, PloS one.
[16] A. Murata,et al. Largely segregated parietofrontal connections linking rostral intraparietal cortex (areas AIP and VIP) and the ventral premotor cortex (areas F5 and F4) , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[17] H. Bekkering,et al. Online prediction of others’ actions: the contribution of the target object, action context and movement kinematics , 2012, Psychological research.
[18] Karl Verfaillie,et al. Perception of biological motion: A stimulus set of human point-light actions , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[19] M. Shiffrar,et al. Detecting deception in a bluffing body: The role of expertise , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[20] M. Shiffrar,et al. Recognizing people from their movement. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[21] U. Castiello,et al. Cues to intention: The role of movement information , 2011, Cognition.
[22] M. Santello,et al. Effects of end-goal on hand shaping. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[23] B. Abernethy,et al. Pickup of Essential Kinematics Underpins Expert Perception of Movement Patterns , 2007, Journal of motor behavior.
[24] Francesca Ugolotti Serventi,et al. Ventral premotor and inferior parietal cortices make distinct contribution to action organization and intention understanding. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.
[25] M. Jeannerod,et al. The motor theory of social cognition: a critique , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[26] James M. Kilner,et al. More than one pathway to action understanding , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[27] U. Castiello,et al. Does the intention to communicate affect action kinematics? , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.
[28] C. Armbrüster,et al. Movement planning in prehension: do intended actions influence the initial reach and grasp movement? , 2006, Motor control.
[29] Pierre Jacob,et al. How from action-mirroring to intention-ascription? , 2013, Consciousness and Cognition.
[30] J. Montepare,et al. Impressions of people created by age-related qualities of their gaits. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[31] Leonardo Fogassi,et al. Neurophysiological bases underlying the organization of intentional actions and the understanding of others’ intention , 2013, Consciousness and Cognition.
[32] R. Blake,et al. Perception of human motion. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.
[33] A. Goldman,et al. Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[34] Cristina Becchio,et al. Communicative Interactions Improve Visual Detection of Biological Motion , 2011, PloS one.
[35] M. Iacoboni,et al. Getting a grip on other minds: Mirror neurons, intention understanding, and cognitive empathy , 2006, Social Neuroscience.
[36] K. Ochsner,et al. The Need for a Cognitive Neuroscience of Naturalistic Social Cognition , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[37] J. Searle. Intentionality: Name index , 1983 .
[38] J. Mazziotta,et al. Grasping the Intentions of Others with One's Own Mirror Neuron System , 2005, PLoS biology.
[39] G. Johansson. Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .
[40] D. Perrett,et al. Responses of Anterior Superior Temporal Polysensory (STPa) Neurons to Biological Motion Stimuli , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[41] B. Abernethy,et al. Expertise and Attunement to Kinematic Constraints , 2008, Perception.
[42] Marco Santello,et al. The effects of task and content on digit placement on a bottle , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[43] R. Blake,et al. Brain Areas Involved in Perception of Biological Motion , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[44] K. Verfaillie,et al. The Second-Agent Effect: Communicative Gestures Increase the Likelihood of Perceiving a Second Agent , 2011, PloS one.
[45] G. Vingerhoets,et al. Multifocal intraparietal activation during discrimination of action intention in observed tool grasping , 2010, Neuroscience.
[46] J. Cutting,et al. Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display , 1977 .
[47] Frank E. Pollick,et al. Neural Substrates for Action Understanding at Different Description Levels in the Human Brain , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[48] Sonja Stork,et al. Subsequent actions influence motor control parameters of a current grasping action , 2008, RO-MAN 2008 - The 17th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication.
[49] A. Clark. Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[50] J. Montepare,et al. Impressions of people created by age-related qualities of their gaits. , 1988 .
[51] Cristina Becchio,et al. Inferring intentions from biological motion: A stimulus set of point-light communicative interactions , 2010, Behavior research methods.
[52] Luca Turella,et al. An object for an action, the same object for other actions: effects on hand shaping , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.
[53] Peter Neri,et al. Meaningful interactions can enhance visual discrimination of human agents , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[54] L. Fogassi,et al. Selectivity for grip type and action goal in macaque inferior parietal and ventral premotor grasping neurons. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.
[55] Karl J. Friston,et al. The mirror-neuron system: a Bayesian perspective. , 2007, Neuroreport.
[56] U. Castiello,et al. Modulation of the action control system by social intention: unexpected social requests override preplanned action. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[57] A. Young,et al. Emotion Perception from Dynamic and Static Body Expressions in Point-Light and Full-Light Displays , 2004, Perception.
[58] V. Manera,et al. Cooperation or competition? Discriminating between social intentions by observing prehensile movements , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.