Using sensory discrimination in a foraging-style task to evaluate human upper-limb sensorimotor performance

Object stiffness discrimination is fundamental to shaping the way we interact with our environment. Investigating the sensorimotor mechanisms underpinning stiffness discrimination may help further our understanding of healthy and sensory-impaired upper limb function. We developed a metric that leverages sensory discrimination techniques and a foraging-based analysis to characterize participant accuracy and discrimination processes of sensorimotor control. Our metric required searching and discriminating two variants of test-object: rubber blocks and spring cells, which emphasized cutaneous-force and proprioceptive feedback, respectively. We measured the number of test-objects handled, selection accuracy, and foraging duration. These values were used to derive six indicators of performance. We observed higher discrimination accuracies, with quicker search and handling durations, for blocks compared to spring cells. Correlative analyses of accuracy, error rates, and foraging times suggested that the block and spring variants were, in fact, unique sensory tasks. These results provide evidence that our metric is sensitive to the contributions of sensory feedback, motor control, and task performance strategy, and will likely be effective in further characterizing the impact of sensory feedback on motor control in healthy and sensory-impaired populations.

[1]  Mewa Singh,et al.  Extractive foraging and tool-aided behaviors in the wild Nicobar long-tailed macaque (Macaca fascicularis umbrosus) , 2018, Primates.

[2]  Adam Kirton,et al.  Kinesthetic deficits after perinatal stroke: robotic measurement in hemiparetic children , 2017, Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation.

[3]  Matthew Fielding,et al.  ‘Disrupting the Optimal Forager’: Predictive Risk Mapping and Domestic Burglary Reduction in Trafford, Greater Manchester , 2012 .

[4]  A. Goodwin,et al.  Perceived roughness of a grating: correlation with responses of mechanoreceptive afferents innervating the monkey's fingerpad , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[5]  Kevin M. Passino,et al.  Generalizing foraging theory for analysis and design , 2011, Int. J. Robotics Res..

[6]  W. B. Whalley Evaluating student assessments: the use of optimal foraging theory , 2016 .

[7]  Mahboubeh Parastarfeizabadi,et al.  Advances in closed-loop deep brain stimulation devices , 2017, Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation.

[8]  Per E. Roland,et al.  Somatosensory detection of microgeometry, macrogeometry and kinesthesia in man , 1987, Brain Research Reviews.

[9]  A. M. Edwards,et al.  Revisiting Lévy flight search patterns of wandering albatrosses, bumblebees and deer , 2007, Nature.

[10]  George L. Farnsworth,et al.  NUMERICAL DISCRIMINATION BY WILD NORTHERN MOCKINGBIRDS , 2006 .

[11]  G. Pyke Optimal Foraging Theory: A Critical Review , 1984 .

[12]  M. Levin,et al.  Motor learning in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy and the role of sensation in short‐term motor training of goal‐directed reaching , 2013, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[13]  B. Bassler,et al.  Quorum sensing: cell-to-cell communication in bacteria. , 2005, Annual review of cell and developmental biology.

[14]  R. Hughes,et al.  ENERGY MAXIMIZATION IN THE DIET OF THE SHORE CRAB, CARCINUS MAENAS , 1978 .

[15]  Ofer Feinerman,et al.  Ant groups optimally amplify the effect of transiently informed individuals , 2015, Nature Communications.

[16]  P. Sandstrom Scholars as Subsistence Foragers , 2005 .

[17]  Amanda W. Y. Tan From Play to Proficiency: The Ontogeny of Stone-Tool Use in Coastal-Foraging Long-Tailed Macaques (Macaca fascicularis) From a Comparative Perception-Action Perspective , 2017, Journal of comparative psychology.

[18]  R. Johansson,et al.  Tactile Sensory Control of Object Manipulation in Humans , 2020, The Senses: A Comprehensive Reference.

[19]  I. Tibbetts,et al.  Foraging behaviour and prey discrimination in the bluespotted maskray Dasyatis kuhlii , 2008 .

[20]  Jacqueline S. Hebert,et al.  Case report of modified Box and Blocks test with motion capture to measure prosthetic function. , 2012, Journal of rehabilitation research and development.

[21]  Elad Schneidman,et al.  Correction: High-order social interactions in groups of mice , 2014, eLife.

[22]  P. A. Prince,et al.  Lévy flight search patterns of wandering albatrosses , 1996, Nature.

[23]  J. Emlen The Role of Time and Energy in Food Preference , 1966, The American Naturalist.

[24]  C. S. Holling Some Characteristics of Simple Types of Predation and Parasitism , 1959, The Canadian Entomologist.

[25]  D. Scheel,et al.  Profitability, encounter rates, and prey choice of African lions , 1993 .

[26]  C. Ghez,et al.  Loss of proprioception produces deficits in interjoint coordination. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  K O Johnson,et al.  Tactile spatial resolution. I. Two-point discrimination, gap detection, grating resolution, and letter recognition. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  John R. Krebs,et al.  Optimal Foraging and Cryptic Prey , 1980 .

[29]  L. Carey,et al.  Sensory loss in stroke patients: effective training of tactile and proprioceptive discrimination. , 1993, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[30]  D. Coltman Evolutionary genetics: Differentiation by dispersal , 2005, Nature.

[31]  Graham H. Pyke,et al.  Optimal Foraging: A Selective Review of Theory and Tests , 1977, The Quarterly Review of Biology.

[32]  Shaun P Jackson,et al.  The class II PI 3-kinase, PI3KC2α, links platelet internal membrane structure to shear-dependent adhesive function , 2015, Nature Communications.

[33]  J. F. Chandler,et al.  Influence of Resource Availability on the Foraging Strategies of the Triangle Butterflyfish Chaetodon triangulum in the Maldives , 2016, PloS one.

[34]  Derick T Wade,et al.  The Rivermead Assessment of Somatosensory Performance (RASP): standardization and reliability data , 2002, Clinical rehabilitation.

[35]  A. Guclu-Gunduz,et al.  Upper extremity function and its relation with hand sensation and upper extremity strength in patients with multiple sclerosis. , 2012, NeuroRehabilitation.

[36]  Kenneth C. Catania,et al.  Asymptotic prey profitability drives star-nosed moles to the foraging speed limit , 2005, Nature.

[37]  Elad Schneidman,et al.  High-order social interactions in groups of mice , 2013, eLife.

[38]  Adi Shklarsh,et al.  Information socialtaxis and efficient collective behavior emerging in groups of information-seeking agents , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[39]  Sandra Parker,et al.  The relationship between Shore hardness of elastomeric dental materials and Young's modulus. , 2009, Dental Materials.

[40]  S. Garland,et al.  CpG Methylation Analysis of HPV16 in Laser Capture Microdissected Archival Tissue and Whole Tissue Sections from High Grade Anal Squamous Intraepithelial Lesions: A Potential Disease Biomarker , 2016, PloS one.

[41]  Sarah F Tyson,et al.  Sensory Loss in Hospital-Admitted People With Stroke: Characteristics, Associated Factors, and Relationship With Function , 2008, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[42]  R. Ware,et al.  Impact of tactile dysfunction on upper-limb motor performance in children with unilateral cerebral palsy. , 2012, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[43]  G. Parisi,et al.  Scale-free correlations in starling flocks , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[44]  L A Jones,et al.  Tactile sensory impairments and prehensile function in subjects with left-hemisphere cerebral lesions. , 1994, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[45]  Mai Dwairy,et al.  The application of foraging theory to the information searching behaviour of general practitioners , 2011, BMC family practice.

[46]  N. Daw,et al.  Learning the opportunity cost of time in a patch-foraging task , 2015, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.

[47]  Diane L Damiano,et al.  Tactile sensory abilities in cerebral palsy: deficits in roughness and object discrimination , 2008, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[48]  J. Staddon,et al.  Searching for Cryptic Prey: The Effect of Search Rate , 1983, The American Naturalist.

[49]  A. Houston,et al.  Optimal prey choice and discrimination time in the great tit (Parus major L.) , 1980, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[50]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Tactual discrimination of softness. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  R. Hughes Optimal Diets under the Energy Maximization Premise: The Effects of Recognition Time and Learning , 1979, The American Naturalist.

[52]  R. Macarthur,et al.  On Optimal Use of a Patchy Environment , 1966, The American Naturalist.

[53]  I. Couzin,et al.  Shared decision-making drives collective movement in wild baboons , 2015, Science.

[54]  E. Charnov Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. , 1976, Theoretical population biology.

[55]  G. Viswanathan,et al.  Lévy flights and superdiffusion in the context of biological encounters and random searches , 2008 .