Measuring spontaneous and automatic processing of magnitude and parity information of Arabic digits by frequency-tagging EEG
暂无分享,去创建一个
Mathieu Guillaume | Alexandre Poncin | Christine Schiltz | Amandine Van Rinsveld | C. Schiltz | Amandine Van Rinsveld | M. Guillaume | Alexandre Poncin | Mathieu Guillaume
[1] Jonathan Evans. On the resolution of conflict in dual process theories of reasoning , 2007 .
[2] Justin M. Ales,et al. The steady-state visual evoked potential in vision research: A review. , 2015, Journal of vision.
[3] G. Denes,et al. A specific deficit for numbers in a case of dense acalculia. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[4] F. Restle. Speed of Adding and Comparing Numbers. , 1970 .
[5] Charles Tijus,et al. Young children’s mapping between arrays, number words, and digits , 2013, Cognition.
[6] W. Schwarz,et al. Searching for the functional locus of the SNARC effect: Evidence for a response-related origin , 2005, Memory & cognition.
[7] M. Sugishita,et al. Isolated acalculia due to left parietal lesion. , 1994, Archives of neurology.
[8] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[9] D. Berch,et al. Extracting parity and magnitude from Arabic numerals: developmental changes in number processing and mental representation. , 1999, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[10] N. Squires,et al. Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[11] V. Csépe,et al. The speed of magnitude processing and executive functions in controlled and automatic number comparison in children: an electro-encephalography study , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Functions.
[12] Daniel Ansari,et al. The role of the left intraparietal sulcus in the relationship between symbolic number processing and children's arithmetic competence , 2012, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.
[13] M. Fayol,et al. When plausibility judgments supersede fact retrieval: The example of the odd-even effect on product verification , 1995, Memory & cognition.
[14] H. Nuerk,et al. Automatic place-value activation in magnitude-irrelevant parity judgement , 2019, Psychological research.
[15] Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al. Chapter 12 Cognitive Number Processing: An Encoding-Complex Perspective , 1992 .
[16] C. Schiltz,et al. Developing number-space associations: SNARC effects using a color discrimination task in 5-year-olds. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[17] Wim Fias,et al. The Heterogeneous Nature of Number–Space Interactions , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[18] Daniel Ansari,et al. Neural correlates of symbolic number processing in children and adults , 2005, Neuroreport.
[19] E. M. Duncan,et al. Isolating the effects of symbolic distance, and semantic congruity in comparative judgments: An additive-factors analysis , 1980, Memory & cognition.
[20] Yukari Okamoto,et al. First graders' cognitive representation of number and understanding of place value: Cross-national comparisons: France, Japan, Korea, Sweden, and the United States. , 1993 .
[21] Dale J Cohen,et al. Integers do not automatically activate their quantity representation , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[22] A. Henik,et al. The contribution of fish studies to the “number sense” debate , 2016, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[23] Bruno Rossion,et al. Left cortical specialization for visual letter strings predicts rudimentary knowledge of letter-sound association in preschoolers , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[24] Daniel Algom,et al. Comparative judgment of numerosity and numerical magnitude: attention preempts automaticity. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[25] S. Dehaene,et al. Imaging unconscious semantic priming , 1998, Nature.
[26] Jonathan Evans. Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.
[27] Yang Seok Cho,et al. Polarity correspondence: A general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.
[28] M. H. Fischer,et al. Number processing induces spatial performance biases , 2001, Neurology.
[29] M. Posner,et al. Brain mechanisms of quantity are similar in 5-year-old children and adults. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[30] A. Kleinschmidt,et al. A Supramodal Number Representation in Human Intraparietal Cortex , 2003, Neuron.
[31] Christine Schiltz,et al. Lateralized Neural Responses to Letters and Digits in First Graders , 2019, Child development.
[32] S Dehaene,et al. Attention, automaticity, and levels of representation in number processing. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[33] W. Fias,et al. How Does Working Memory Enable Number-Induced Spatial Biases? , 2016, Front. Psychol..
[34] A. Norcia,et al. An objective index of individual face discrimination in the right occipito-temporal cortex by means of fast periodic oddball stimulation , 2014, Neuropsychologia.
[35] Wim Fias,et al. The temporary nature of number-space interactions. , 2016, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[36] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Neural mechanisms of attentional shifts due to irrelevant spatial and numerical cues , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[37] Nina Bien,et al. Automatic and Intentional Number Processing Both Rely on Intact Right Parietal Cortex: A Combined fMRI and Neuronavigated TMS Study , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[38] Bruno Rossion,et al. A rapid, objective and implicit measure of visual quantity discrimination , 2018, Neuropsychologia.
[39] Jean-Philippe van Dijck,et al. Numbers are associated with different types of spatial information depending on the task , 2009, Cognition.
[40] H. Nuerk,et al. A Mental Odd-Even Continuum Account: Some Numbers May Be “More Odd” Than Others and Some Numbers May Be “More Even” Than Others , 2018, Front. Psychol..
[41] Jean-Philippe van Dijck,et al. A working memory account for spatial–numerical associations , 2011, Cognition.
[42] P. Lemaire,et al. Age-related differences in automatic stimulus-response associations: Insights from young and older adults’ parity judgments , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[43] Daniel Ansari,et al. How do symbolic and non-symbolic numerical magnitude processing skills relate to individual differences in children's mathematical skills? A review of evidence from brain and behavior , 2013, Trends in Neuroscience and Education.
[44] Margot J. Taylor,et al. Is 2+2=4? Meta-analyses of brain areas needed for numbers and calculations , 2011, NeuroImage.
[45] Avishai Henik,et al. Automatic and intentional processing of numerical information , 1992 .
[46] Bruno Rossion,et al. Understanding individual face discrimination by means of fast periodic visual stimulation , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.
[47] B. Rossion,et al. Supra-additive contribution of shape and surface information to individual face discrimination as revealed by fast periodic visual stimulation. , 2014, Journal of vision.
[48] B. Rossion,et al. The effect of parametric stimulus size variation on individual face discrimination indexed by fast periodic visual stimulation , 2014, BMC Neuroscience.
[49] L E Krueger,et al. Why 2 + 2 = 5 looks so wrong: On the odd-even rule in sum verification , 1984, Memory & cognition.
[50] K Moeller,et al. Early place-value understanding as a precursor for later arithmetic performance--a longitudinal study on numerical development. , 2011, Research in developmental disabilities.
[51] S. Dehaene,et al. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .
[52] L Girelli,et al. The development of automaticity in accessing number magnitude. , 2000, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[53] W. Fias,et al. Semantic Influences on Feature-Based Attention Due to Overlap of Neural Circuits , 2002, Cortex.
[54] Yi-Fang Hsu,et al. The time course of symbolic number adaptation: Oscillatory EEG activity and event-related potential analysis , 2012, NeuroImage.
[55] S. Dehaene,et al. Differential Contributions of the Left and Right Inferior Parietal Lobules to Number Processing , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[56] S. Dehaene,et al. THREE PARIETAL CIRCUITS FOR NUMBER PROCESSING , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[57] Valérie Dormal,et al. Processing numerosity, length and duration in a three-dimensional Stroop-like task: towards a gradient of processing automaticity? , 2013, Psychological research.
[58] J. Tzelgov,et al. Automatic comparisons of artificial digits never compared: learning linear ordering relations. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[59] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Cerebral Pathways for Calculation: Double Dissociation between Rote Verbal and Quantitative Knowledge of Arithmetic , 1997, Cortex.
[60] ROBERT S. MOYER,et al. Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.
[61] W. Gevers,et al. The SNARC effect does not imply a mental number line , 2008, Cognition.
[62] A. Norcia,et al. An objective signature for visual binding of face parts in the human brain. , 2013, Journal of vision.
[63] Nobuyuki Fujisawa,et al. Occurrence of asymmetrical flow pattern behind an orifice in a circular pipe , 2011, J. Vis..
[64] K. Miller. Chapter 1 What A Number is: Mathematical Foundations and Developing Number Concepts , 1992 .
[65] M. Brysbaert,et al. Are Arabic numerals processed as pictures in a Stroop interference task? , 2001, Psychological research.
[66] François Jouen,et al. Do young children acquire number words through subitizing or counting , 2004 .
[67] Bruno Rossion,et al. A robust index of lexical representation in the left occipito-temporal cortex as evidenced by EEG responses to fast periodic visual stimulation , 2015, Neuropsychologia.
[68] Arava Y. Kallai,et al. The place-value of a digit in multi-digit numbers is processed automatically. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[69] Wim Fias,et al. Numbers and space: a computational model of the SNARC effect. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[70] M. Thioux,et al. Neuroanatomical Substrates of Arabic Number Processing, Numerical Comparison, and Simple Addition: A PET Study , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[71] Jonathan Evans,et al. Science Perspectives on Psychological , 2022 .
[72] J. Ridley. Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .
[73] A. Henik,et al. Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks , 1982, Memory & cognition.
[74] Jyotsna Vaid,et al. Visual field asymmetries in numerical size comparisons of digits, words, and signs , 1989, Brain and Language.
[75] Valérie Goffaux,et al. Shifts of spatial attention cued by irrelevant numbers: Electrophysiological evidence from a target discrimination task , 2015 .
[76] Chuansheng Chen,et al. Development of spatial representation of numbers: a study of the SNARC effect in Chinese children. , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[77] D G Pelli,et al. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[78] Bruno Rossion,et al. Rapid Objective Assessment of Contrast Sensitivity and Visual Acuity With Sweep Visual Evoked Potentials and an Extended Electrode Array. , 2018, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.
[79] Rochel Gelman,et al. The Child's Representation of Number: A Multidimensional Scaling Analysis. , 1983 .
[80] Avishai Henik,et al. Automatic activation of internal magnitudes: a study of developmental dyscalculia. , 2005, Neuropsychology.
[81] Daniel Ansari,et al. Common and segregated neural pathways for the processing of symbolic and nonsymbolic numerical magnitude: An fMRI study , 2010, NeuroImage.
[82] Bruno Rossion,et al. Uncovering the neural magnitude and spatio-temporal dynamics of natural image categorization in a fast visual stream , 2016, Neuropsychologia.
[83] R. Goebel,et al. Attentional shifts induced by uninformative number symbols modulate neural activity in human occipital cortex , 2012, Neuropsychologia.
[84] Lisa K Fazio,et al. Relations of different types of numerical magnitude representations to each other and to mathematics achievement. , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[85] Derek Besner,et al. Ideographic and alphabetic processing in skilled reading of English , 1979, Neuropsychologia.
[86] Nicolas Costes,et al. Task-independent semantic activation for numbers and animals. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[87] André Mouraux,et al. Fast periodic visual stimulation to study tool-selective processing in the human brain , 2018, Experimental Brain Research.
[88] Dana Ganor-Stern,et al. Automaticity in Processing Ordinal Information , 2004 .
[89] Denis G. Pelli,et al. ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.
[90] Frank Domahs,et al. Grammatical number elicits SNARC and MARC effects as a function of task demands , 2015, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[91] T. Surrey,et al. Complementary activities of TPX2 and chTOG constitute an efficient importin-regulated microtubule nucleation module , 2015, Nature Cell Biology.
[92] Avishai Henik,et al. Virtual Dyscalculia Induced by Parietal-Lobe TMS Impairs Automatic Magnitude Processing , 2007, Current Biology.
[93] Julie L. Booth,et al. Numerical magnitude representations influence arithmetic learning. , 2008, Child development.
[94] S. Dehaene. Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.
[95] Melissa E. Libertus,et al. Electrophysiological evidence for notation independence in numerical processing , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Functions.
[96] B. Rossion,et al. The non-linear development of the right hemispheric specialization for human face perception , 2017, Neuropsychologia.
[97] M. Inglis,et al. An Introduction to Mathematical Cognition , 2018 .
[98] M. Zorzi,et al. Storage and retrieval of addition facts: The role of number comparison , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[99] Freya Acar,et al. A Working Memory Account of the Interaction between Numbers and Spatial Attention , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.