Motor equivalent control of the center of mass in response to support surface perturbations

To claim that the center of mass (CM) of the body is a controlled variable of the postural system is difficult to verify experimentally. In this report, a new variant of the method of the uncontrolled manifold (UCM) hypothesis was used to evaluate CM control in response to an abrupt surface perturbation during stance. Subjects stood upright on a support surface that was displaced in the posterior direction. Support surface translations between 0.03 and 0.12 m, each lasting for 275 ms, were presented randomly. The UCM corresponding to all possible combinations of joints that are equivalent with respect to producing the average pre-perturbation anterior–posterior position of the center of mass (CMAP) were linearly estimated for each trial. At each point in time thereafter, the difference between the current joint configuration and the average pre-perturbation joint configuration was computed. This joint difference vector was then projected onto the pre-perturbation UCM as a measure of motor equivalence, and onto its complementary subspace, which represents joint combinations that lead to a different CMAP position. A similar analysis was performed related to control of the trunk’s spatial orientation. The extent to which the joint velocity vector acted to stabilize the CMAP position was also examined. Excursions of the hip and ankle joints both increased linearly with perturbation magnitude. The configuration of joints at each instance during the perturbation differed from the mean configuration prior to the perturbation, as evidenced by the joint difference vector. Most of this joint difference vector was consistent, however, with the average pre-perturbation CMAP position rather than leading to a different CMAP position. This was not the case, however, when performing this analysis with respect to the UCM corresponding to the control of the pre-perturbation trunk orientation. The projection of the instantaneous joint velocity vector also was found to lie primarily in the UCM corresponding to the pre-perturbation CMAP position, indicating that joint motion was damped in directions leading to a change away from the pre-perturbation CMAP position. These results provide quantitative support for the argument that the CM position is a planned variable of the postural system and that its control is achieved through selective, motor equivalent changes in the joint configuration in response to support surface perturbations. The results suggest that the nervous system accomplishes postural control by a control strategy that considers all DOFs. This strategy presumably resists combinations of DOFs that affect the stability of important task-relevant variables (CMAP position) while, to a large extent, freeing from control combinations of those DOFs that have no effect on the task-relevant variables (Schöner in Ecol Psychol 8:291–314, 1995).

[1]  J. Massion Movement, posture and equilibrium: Interaction and coordination , 1992, Progress in Neurobiology.

[2]  R. Peterka Sensorimotor integration in human postural control. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  M. Latash,et al.  Muscle synergies during shifts of the center of pressure by standing persons , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  John P. Scholz,et al.  Joint coordination during quiet stance: effects of vision , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  Tim Kiemel,et al.  A unified view of quiet and perturbed stance: simultaneous co-existing excitable modes , 2005, Neuroscience Letters.

[6]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  The uncontrolled manifold concept: identifying control variables for a functional task , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  Identifying the control structure of multijoint coordination during pistol shooting , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  Marc H. Raibert A Model for Motor Control and Learning , 1977, IJCAI.

[9]  F. Horak,et al.  Influence of stimulus parameters on human postural responses. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  G. Schöner,et al.  Effects of varying task constraints on solutions to joint coordination in a sit-to-stand task , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  V. S. Gurfinkel,et al.  Kinesthetic reference for human orthograde posture , 1995, Neuroscience.

[12]  Hélène Corriveau,et al.  Postural stability in the elderly: empirical confirmation of a theoretical model. , 2004, Archives of gerontology and geriatrics.

[13]  M. Raibert Motor Control and Learning by the State Space Model , 1977 .

[14]  D. Winter,et al.  Stiffness control of balance in quiet standing. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  F. Horak,et al.  Central programming of postural movements: adaptation to altered support-surface configurations. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  F E Zajac,et al.  Ankle and hip postural strategies defined by joint torques. , 1999, Gait & posture.

[17]  F. Horak,et al.  Influence of central set on human postural responses. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Kinematic and electromyographic responses to perturbation of a rapid grasp. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  Fay B. Horak,et al.  Postural Adaptation for Altered Environments, Tasks, and Intentions , 2000 .

[20]  Frans C. T. van der Helm,et al.  An adaptive model of sensory integration in a dynamic environment applied to human stance control , 2001, Biological Cybernetics.

[21]  P. Morasso,et al.  Body sway during quiet standing: is it the residual chattering of an intermittent stabilization process? , 2005, Human movement science.

[22]  F. Horak,et al.  Postural feedback responses scale with biomechanical constraints in human standing , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  E. Todorov Optimality principles in sensorimotor control , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[24]  J. Kelso,et al.  Functionally specific articulatory cooperation following jaw perturbations during speech: evidence for coordinative structures. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  G. Schöner Recent Developments and Problems in Human Movement Science and Their Conceptual Implications , 1995 .

[26]  M. Latash,et al.  Muscle modes during shifts of the center of pressure by standing persons: effect of instability and additional support , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  M. Latash,et al.  Two kinematic synergies in voluntary whole-body movements during standing. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  S. Wise,et al.  The Acquisition of Motor Behavior in Vertebrates , 1996 .

[29]  Giancarlo Ferrigno,et al.  Absence of center of mass control for leg abduction in long-term weightlessness in humans , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[30]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  Goal-equivalent joint coordination in pointing: affect of vision and arm dominance. , 2002, Motor control.

[31]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  Differential joint coordination in the tasks of standing up and sitting down. , 2002, Journal of electromyography and kinesiology : official journal of the International Society of Electrophysiological Kinesiology.

[32]  Tim Kiemel,et al.  Controlling human upright posture: velocity information is more accurate than position or acceleration. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  Tim Kiemel,et al.  Slow dynamics of postural sway are in the feedback loop. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  F E Zajac,et al.  Human standing posture: multi-joint movement strategies based on biomechanical constraints. , 1993, Progress in brain research.

[35]  M. Latash,et al.  Motor Control Strategies Revealed in the Structure of Motor Variability , 2002, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[36]  B. E. Maki,et al.  Change-in-support balance reactions in older persons: an emerging research area of clinical importance. , 2005, Neurologic clinics.

[37]  J. Abbs,et al.  Control of complex motor gestures: orofacial muscle responses to load perturbations of lip during speech. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  William H Gage,et al.  Kinematic and kinetic validity of the inverted pendulum model in quiet standing. , 2004, Gait & posture.

[39]  L. Nashner,et al.  The organization of human postural movements: A formal basis and experimental synthesis , 1985, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[40]  A. Kuo A least-squares estimation approach to improving the precision of inverse dynamics computations. , 1998, Journal of biomechanical engineering.

[41]  David A. Winter,et al.  Biomechanics and Motor Control of Human Movement , 1990 .