The Development of Children's Adaptive Expertise in the Number Domain 20 to 100

The aim of the study was to analyze the development of children's adaptive expertise in computing sums and differences up to 100.We defined the adaptive nature of children's strategy choices on the basis of problem (addition, subtraction), achievement, and strategy performance (accuracy, speed). Sixty-nine 2nd graders of high, above-average, or below-average mathematical achievement level solved 4 types of additions and subtractions before and after regular classroom instruction in the topic. In both assessments, children answered the problems in a choice condition, in which they could choose between the jump strategy (45 + 28 = __; 45 + 20 = 65, 65 + 8 = 73) and the split strategy (45 + 28 = __; 40 + 20 = 60, 5 + 8 = 13, 60 + 13 = 73) on each problem, and in two no-choice conditions, where they had to solve all problems with either the jump or the split strategy. Our results revealed differences in children's strategy choices on both assessments. The choices of high achievers indicated that they fit their strategies to problem and strategy performance characteristics. In contrast, above-average and below-average achievers' choices were unrelated to those characteristics. These results are interpreted in terms of the viability of different instructional approaches to enhance the acquisition of adaptive expertise, specifically, conceptual and investigative approaches.

[1]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Whole number concepts and operations , 2007 .

[2]  Robert E. Reys,et al.  Mental Computation Performance and Strategy Use of Japanese Students in Grades 2, 4, 6, and 8. , 1995 .

[3]  P. Cobb,et al.  Sociomathematical Norms, Argumentation, and Autonomy in Mathematics. , 1996 .

[4]  A. W. Blöte,et al.  Students' flexibility in solving two-digit addition and subtraction problems: Instruction effects. , 2001 .

[5]  Athanassios Raftopoulos,et al.  Cognitive Developmental Change: Theories, Models and Measurement , 2005 .

[6]  Meindert Beishuizen,et al.  Mental Strategies and Materials or Models for Addition and Subtraction Up to 100 in Dutch Second Grades. , 1993 .

[7]  Mark H. Ashcraft,et al.  Is It Farfetched That Some of Us Remember Our Arithmetic Facts , 1985 .

[8]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Strategy development in children with mathematical disabilities: insights from the choice/no-choice method and the chronological-age/ ability-level-match design. , 2004, Journal of learning disabilities.

[9]  Robert E. Reys Helping Children Learn Mathematics , 1984 .

[10]  G. D. Logan Task Switching , 2022 .

[11]  Arthur J. Baroody The development of adaptive expertise and flexibility: The integration of conceptual and procedural knowledge , 2003 .

[12]  L. Beach,et al.  A Contingency Model for the Selection of Decision Strategies , 1978 .

[13]  Marja van den Heuvel-Panhuizen,et al.  Children learn mathematics : a learning-teaching trajectory with intermediate attainment targets for calculation with whole numbers in primary school , 2001 .

[14]  T. P. Carpenter,et al.  Children's Conceptual Structures for Multidigit Numbers and Methods of Multidigit Addition and Subtraction. , 1997 .

[15]  Arthur J. Baroody,et al.  Two perspectives on addition development , 2003 .

[16]  Karen C. Fuson,et al.  Research on whole number addition and subtraction. , 1992 .

[17]  Meindert Beishuizen,et al.  MENTAL ARITHMETIC AND STRATEGY USE WITH INDIRECT NUMBER PROBLEMS UP TO ONE HUNDRED , 1997 .

[18]  G. Wentworth A Mental Arithmetic , 2008 .

[19]  John W. Payne,et al.  The adaptive decision maker: Name index , 1993 .

[20]  Douglas A. Grouws,et al.  Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning: A project of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. , 1992 .

[21]  Thomas J. Cooper,et al.  Factors affecting the process of proficient mental addition and subtraction: case studies of flexible and inflexible computers , 2004 .

[22]  최영한,et al.  미국 NCTM의 Principles and Standards for School Mathematics에 나타난 수학과 교수,학습의 이론 , 2002 .

[23]  Ian Thompson,et al.  Mental Calculation Strategies for Addition and Subtraction , 1999 .

[24]  A. Treffers,et al.  The empty number line in Dutch second grades: Realistic versus gradual program design , 1998 .

[25]  A. Su,et al.  The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics , 1932, The Mathematical Gazette.

[26]  L. Verschaffel,et al.  Simple Addition Strategies in a First-Grade Class With Multiple Strategy Instruction , 2005 .

[27]  R. Littell SAS System for Mixed Models , 1996 .

[28]  Ian Putt,et al.  A model for nurturing and assessing multidigit number sense among first grade children , 1994 .

[29]  A. W. Blöte,et al.  Mental computation and conceptual understanding , 2000 .

[30]  G. Hatano,et al.  Commentary: Reconceptualizing School Learning Using Insight From Expertise Research , 2003 .

[31]  Eric J. Johnson,et al.  The adaptive decision maker , 1993 .

[32]  A. Baroody Children's Mathematical Thinking: A Developmental Framework for Preschool, Primary, and Special Education Teachers , 1987 .

[33]  J. Hiebert Conceptual and procedural knowledge : the case of mathematics , 1987 .

[34]  Christoph Selter,et al.  Building on Children's Mathematics - a Teaching Experiment in Grade Three , 1998 .

[35]  R. Siegler,et al.  Older and younger adults' strategy choices in multiplication: testing predictions of ASCM using the choice/no-choice method. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[36]  Willem Jongman,et al.  Adding it up , 1988 .

[37]  Susan B. Empson,et al.  A longitudinal study of invention and understanding in children's multidigit addition and subtraction , 1998 .

[38]  Ann Dowker,et al.  The development of arithmetic concepts and skills: Constructing adaptive expertise , 2003 .

[39]  James Hiebert,et al.  Instruction, Understanding, and Skill in Multidigit Addition and Subtraction , 1996 .