Cultural differences in learning novel words in an attention-following versus attention-directing style

Differences in attention regulation style and their affect on word learning were explored in two different cultures, first-generation Mexican and American. Ten toddlers, five American and five Mexican-immigrant, ages 15 – 21 months, were taught nonsense words in attention-directing and attention-following styles over a two-week period. Results showed that the Mexican-immigrant children learned more words in an attention-directing style than in an attention-following style in Week One. No differences were found in vocabulary in the American children in Week One or Week Two. However, there was an overall trend that the American children learned more words than the Mexican-immigrant children in an attention-following style. These results have implications for clinical intervention.

[1]  B. Everitt,et al.  Statistical methods for rates and proportions , 1973 .

[2]  A. Fernald,et al.  Common themes and cultural variations in Japanese and American mothers' speech to infants. , 1993, Child development.

[3]  B. Rogoff,et al.  Cultural variation in management of attention by children and their caregivers. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[4]  J. Bruner From communication to language—a psychological perspective , 1975, Cognition.

[5]  Debra C Vigil,et al.  Cultural variations in attention regulation: a comparative analysis of British and Chinese-immigrant populations. , 2002, International journal of language & communication disorders.

[6]  M. Steward,et al.  Effect of social distance on teaching strategies of Anglo-American and Mexican-American mothers. , 1974 .

[7]  Philip S. Dale,et al.  Macarthur Communicative Development Inventories , 1992 .

[8]  C. Westby,et al.  Clinical Issues: Caregiver Interaction Style , 2004 .

[9]  J. Farver,et al.  Cultural differences in American and Mexican mother-child pretend play , 1993 .

[10]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Linguistic environment of 1- to 2-year-old twins. , 1986 .

[11]  J. Bruner Child's Talk: Learning to Use Language , 1985 .

[12]  Adele Diamond,et al.  Fast mapping of multiple words: Insights into when ''the information provided'' does and does not equal ''the information perceived'' , 2003 .

[13]  A. Sameroff,et al.  Acculturation and SES Effects on Mexican-American Parents' Concepts of Development. , 1988 .

[14]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Joint attention and early language. , 1986, Child development.

[15]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Joint attention and lexical acquisition style , 1983 .

[16]  E. Hoff-Ginsberg,et al.  Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. , 1991, Child development.

[17]  Robert F. Orlikoff,et al.  Evaluating Research in Communicative Disorders , 1996 .

[18]  B. Rogoff,et al.  Guided participation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. , 1993, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[19]  J. Fleiss,et al.  Statistical methods for rates and proportions , 1973 .

[20]  H. G. Kayser,et al.  Bilingual Speech-Language Pathology: An Hispanic Focus , 1995 .

[21]  C. Westby,et al.  Caregiver Interaction Style , 2004 .

[22]  Philip J. Dunham,et al.  Joint-Attentional States and Lexical Acquisition at 18 Months. , 1993 .

[23]  L. M. Laosa Maternal Teaching Strategies in Chicano Families of Varied Educational and Socioeconomic Levels. , 1978 .

[24]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Mother and Infant Activity and Interaction in Japan and in the United States: II. A Comparative Microanalysis of Naturalistic Exchanges Focused on the Organisation of Infant Attention , 1990 .

[25]  L. M. Laosa Maternal Teaching Strategies in Chicano and Anglo-American Families: The Influence of Culture and Education on Maternal Behavior. , 1980 .