Reasoning on conflicting information: An empirical study of Formal Argumentation

According to the Argumentative Theory, human reasoning has an argumentative function, which consists of devising and evaluating arguments for and against various claims. It is however unclear how humans handle conflicting claims they face in everyday life (i.e., “Bob is telling me that Alice is at the library” vs. “Charles is telling me that Alice is at home”). We here investigate human argumentative reasoning in the light of Formal Argumentation, a research field that develops formal methods to give a normative account of argumentation and reasoning about conflicting information. In Formal Argumentation, multiple argumentation semantics that allow selecting sets of jointly acceptable arguments have been proposed. Nonetheless, it is unclear which of these semantics predicts best how humans evaluate the acceptability of conflicting arguments. We conducted an empirical study in which 130 young adults judged natural language arguments. We instructed them to draw the attack relation between the given arguments and to evaluate the acceptability of each of these arguments. Our results show that human judgments on the existence and directionality of attacks between the arguments conform to theoretical predictions from Formal Argumentation. We further found out that some less well-known argumentation semantics predicted human evaluation better than the most well-known semantics. These findings support the cognitive plausibility of variants of Formal Argumentation and bring new insights into reasoning about conflicting information.

[1]  Anthony Hunter,et al.  Empirical evaluation of abstract argumentation: Supporting the need for bipolar and probabilistic approaches , 2017, Int. J. Approx. Reason..

[2]  Sarit Kraus,et al.  Providing Arguments in Discussions on the Basis of the Prediction of Human Argumentative Behavior , 2016, ACM Trans. Interact. Intell. Syst..

[3]  David A. Sears,et al.  Effects of Small-Group Learning on Transfer: a Meta-Analysis , 2015 .

[4]  D. Kuhn Thinking Together and Alone , 2015 .

[5]  Nava Tintarev,et al.  Formal Arguments, Preferences, and Natural Language Interfaces to Humans: an Empirical Evaluation , 2014, ECAI.

[6]  Henry Prakken,et al.  The ASPIC+ framework for structured argumentation: a tutorial , 2014, Argument Comput..

[7]  Guillermo R. Simari,et al.  Introduction to structured argumentation , 2014, Argument Comput..

[8]  Claudette Cayrol,et al.  Bipolarity in argumentation graphs: Towards a better understanding , 2011, Int. J. Approx. Reason..

[9]  J. Dessalles,et al.  Arguing, reasoning, and the interpersonal (cultural) functions of human consciousness , 2011, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[10]  Mohammed Iqbal Madakkatel,et al.  Behavioral Experiments for Assessing the Abstract Argumentation Semantics of Reinstatement , 2010, Cogn. Sci..

[11]  P. Johnson-Laird INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:Mental models and human reasoning , 2010 .

[12]  Henry Prakken,et al.  An abstract framework for argumentation with structured arguments , 2010, Argument Comput..

[13]  Maria Augustinova Falsification cueing in collective reasoning: Example of the Wason selection task , 2008 .

[14]  Derek C. Penn,et al.  Darwin's mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds , 2008, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[15]  Jonathan Evans,et al.  Reasoning about necessity and possibility : a test of the mental model theory of deduction , 1999 .

[16]  M. Tomasello The Human Adaptation for Culture , 1999 .

[17]  David Moshman Molly Geil,et al.  Collaborative Reasoning: Evidence for Collective Rationality , 1998 .

[18]  Phan Minh Dung,et al.  On the Acceptability of Arguments and its Fundamental Role in Nonmonotonic Reasoning, Logic Programming and n-Person Games , 1995, Artif. Intell..

[19]  M. Azmitia,et al.  Friendship, transactive dialogues, and the development of scientific reasoning , 1993 .

[20]  Rose J. Dimant,et al.  Development of formal reasoning during successive peer interactions. , 1991 .

[21]  Z. Kunda,et al.  The case for motivated reasoning. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.

[22]  John W. Payne,et al.  Contingent decision behavior. , 1982 .

[23]  Marcos Cramer,et al.  Directionality of Attacks in Natural Language Argumentation , 2018, Bridging@IJCAI/ECAI.

[24]  Marcos Cramer,et al.  Empirical Cognitive Study on Abstract Argumentation Semantics , 2018, COMMA.

[25]  Pietro Baroni,et al.  Abstract Argumentation Frameworks and Their Semantics , 2018 .

[26]  Leon van der Torre,et al.  The Principle-Based Approach to Abstract Argumentation Semantics , 2017 .

[27]  Sanjay Modgil,et al.  Preferences and Unrestricted Rebut , 2014, COMMA.

[28]  Pietro Baroni,et al.  Semantics of Abstract Argument Systems , 2009, Argumentation in Artificial Intelligence.

[29]  Keith Stenning,et al.  Human Reasoning and Cognitive Science , 2008 .

[30]  Floris Bex,et al.  Computational Models of Argument, Proceedings of COMMA , 2008, COMMA 2008.

[31]  Jie Bao,et al.  Reasoning , 2008, Encyclopedia of GIS.

[32]  P. Baroni,et al.  Characterizing defeat graphs where argumentation semantics agree , 2007 .

[33]  E. Deci,et al.  Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. , 2000, The American psychologist.

[34]  P. Dillenbourg What do you mean by collaborative learning , 1999 .

[35]  Henry Prakken,et al.  Argument-Based Extended Logic Programming with Defeasible Priorities , 1997, J. Appl. Non Class. Logics.

[36]  S. Asch Studies of independence and conformity: I. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. , 1956 .

[37]  Sociological Studies , 1934, Nature.