Speed Biases With Real-Life Video Clips

We live almost literally immersed in an artificial visual world, especially motion pictures. In this exploratory study, we asked whether the best speed for reproducing a video is its original, shooting speed. By using adjustment and double staircase methods, we examined speed biases in viewing real-life video clips in three experiments, and assessed their robustness by manipulating visual and auditory factors. With the tested stimuli (short clips of human motion, mixed human-physical motion, physical motion and ego-motion), speed underestimation was the rule rather than the exception, although it depended largely on clip content, ranging on average from 2% (ego-motion) to 32% (physical motion). Manipulating display size or adding arbitrary soundtracks did not modify these speed biases. Estimated speed was not correlated with estimated duration of these same video clips. These results indicate that the sense of speed for real-life video clips can be systematically biased, independently of the impression of elapsed time. Measuring subjective visual tempo may integrate traditional methods that assess time perception: speed biases may be exploited to develop a simple, objective test of reality flow, to be used for example in clinical and developmental contexts. From the perspective of video media, measuring speed biases may help to optimize video reproduction speed and validate “natural” video compression techniques based on sub-threshold temporal squeezing.

[1]  M. Shiffrar,et al.  The visual perception of human locomotion. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[2]  Argiro Vatakis,et al.  "Flash" dance: how speed modulates percieved duration in dancers and non-dancers. , 2014, Acta psychologica.

[3]  James R. Kubricht,et al.  Intuitive Physics: Current Research and Controversies , 2017, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  W. Ehrenstein,et al.  Psychophysical Methods , 1999 .

[6]  M. Hoshiyama,et al.  Effects of emotional music on visual processes in inferior temporal area , 2013, Cognitive neuroscience.

[7]  Yi-Huang Su,et al.  Visual Timing of Structured Dance Movements Resembles Auditory Rhythm Perception , 2016, Neural plasticity.

[8]  D. Burr,et al.  Temporal integration of optic flow, measured by contrast and coherence thresholds , 2001, Vision Research.

[9]  A J Ahumada,et al.  Model of human visual-motion sensing. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[10]  M. McCloskey,et al.  Naive physics: the curvilinear impetus principle and its role in interactions with moving objects. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[11]  C. Krumhansl An exploratory study of musical emotions and psychophysiology. , 1997, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[12]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Influence of the size of the field of view on motion perception , 2009, Comput. Graph..

[13]  P. Juslin,et al.  Visual perception of dynamic properties: cue heuristics versus direct-perceptual competence. , 2000, Psychological review.

[14]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Human brain activity time-locked to perceptual event boundaries , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  P Viviani,et al.  The Relationship between Curvature and Velocity in Two-Dimensional Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  Claudio de'Sperati,et al.  Visual tuning to kinematics of biological motion: the role of eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  Stuart Anstis,et al.  Moving objects appear to slow down at low contrasts , 2003, Neural Networks.

[18]  Karl R. Gegenfurtner,et al.  Robust Underestimation of Speed During Driving: A Field Study , 2015, Perception.

[19]  H. Hecht,et al.  Influence of animation on dynamical judgments. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  B. Hu,et al.  The Effects of Music Salience on the Gait Performance of Young Adults. , 2015, Journal of music therapy.

[21]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[22]  Claudio de'Sperati,et al.  Motor imagery and visual event recognition , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  Francesco Lacquaniti,et al.  Effects of speeding up or slowing down animate or inanimate motions on timing , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror neuron system. , 2009, Archives of neurology.

[25]  C. de’Sperati,et al.  Recognizing the motion of a graspable object is guided by handedness , 1997, Neuroreport.

[26]  Claudio de’Sperati,et al.  Low perceptual sensitivity to altered video speed in viewing a soccer match , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[27]  Warren H. Meck,et al.  Bayesian optimization of time perception , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  Volker H. Franz,et al.  When is grasping affected by the Müller-Lyer illusion? A quantitative review , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  Claudio de'Sperati,et al.  Delayed Perceptual Awareness in Rapid Perceptual Decisions , 2011, PloS one.

[30]  Hartwig K. Distler,et al.  Velocity Constancy in a Virtual Reality Environment , 1997, Perception.

[31]  Charles Spence,et al.  Cross-correlation between auditory and visual signals promotes multisensory integration. , 2013, Multisensory research.

[32]  A. Proverbio,et al.  The effect of background music on episodic memory and autonomic responses: listening to emotionally touching music enhances facial memory capacity , 2015, Scientific Reports.

[33]  A. Cowey,et al.  Can spatial and temporal motion integration compensate for deficits in local motion mechanisms? , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Richard A. Bwck Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: A meta-analytic review , 1997 .

[35]  D. Burr,et al.  Motion psychophysics: 1985–2010 , 2011, Vision Research.

[36]  R. Snowden,et al.  Speed perception fogs up as visibility drops , 1998, Nature.

[37]  P. Viviani,et al.  Perceiving and tracking kinesthetic stimuli: further evidence of motor-perceptual interactions. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[38]  Stephen T. Hammett,et al.  Speed can go up as well as down at low contrast: Implications for models of motion perception , 2006, Vision Research.

[39]  Thomas Schäfer,et al.  Changes in the representation of space and time while listening to music , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[40]  Aleksander Väljamäe,et al.  Multisensory Interactions during Motion Perception , 2012 .

[41]  Carlo Robino,et al.  Parsing visual stimuli into temporal units through eye movements , 2012, ETRA.

[42]  Yi-Huang Su,et al.  Hearing the speed: visual motion biases the perception of auditory tempo , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  C. de’Sperati,et al.  Blind Saccades: An Asynchrony between Seeing and Looking , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Sound alters visual motion perception , 1997, Nature.

[45]  S. Crespi,et al.  Spotting expertise in the eyes: billiards knowledge as revealed by gaze shifts in a dynamic visual prediction task. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[46]  J. Pittenger,et al.  Estimation of Pendulum Length from Information in Motion , 1985, Perception.

[47]  Rebecca J Sharman,et al.  Visual adaptation alters the apparent speed of real-world actions , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[48]  Robert J Zatorre,et al.  Musical pleasure and reward: mechanisms and dysfunction , 2015, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[49]  G. Recanzone Auditory influences on visual temporal rate perception. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  D. Burr,et al.  Predictive coding of multisensory timing , 2016, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[51]  Scott W. Brown Time, change, and motion: The effects of stimulus movement on temporal perception , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[52]  Ian M Thornton,et al.  Matching biological motion at extreme distances. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[53]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Noise characteristics and prior expectations in human visual speed perception , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[54]  Kevin Brownlow,et al.  Silent Films–What Was the Right Speed? , 1990 .

[55]  M. McCloskey,et al.  Intuitive physics: the straight-down belief and its origin. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[56]  S. Grondin Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[57]  David Alais,et al.  No direction-specific bimodal facilitation for audiovisual motion detection. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[58]  Clara Casco,et al.  Audiovisual bounce-inducing effect: When sound congruence affects grouping in vision , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[59]  Gabriel Baud-Bovy,et al.  Perceiving and tracking kinesthetic stimuli: further evidence of motor-perceptual interactions. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[60]  Justin L. Gardner,et al.  Cortical Correlates of Human Motion Perception Biases , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[61]  E Castet,et al.  Apparent speed of sampled motion , 1995, Vision Research.

[62]  Lucas Spierer,et al.  Dynamic calibration of our sense of time , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[63]  Antony B. Morland,et al.  Perceptual distortions of speed at low luminance: Evidence inconsistent with a Bayesian account of speed encoding , 2007, Vision Research.

[64]  Henrik Olsson,et al.  Visual perception of dynamic properties: cue heuristics versus direct-perceptual competence. , 2000, Psychological review.

[65]  S. Shimojo,et al.  When Sound Affects Vision: Effects of Auditory Grouping on Visual Motion Perception , 2001, Psychological science.

[66]  Susan J. Galvin,et al.  Type 2 tasks in the theory of signal detectability: Discrimination between correct and incorrect decisions , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[67]  V. Gallese,et al.  Motor cognition and its role in the phylogeny and ontogeny of action understanding. , 2009, Developmental psychology.

[68]  Edward H. Adelson,et al.  Motion illusions as optimal percepts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[69]  Thomas Banton,et al.  The Perception of Walking Speed in a Virtual Environment , 2005, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[70]  Francesco Lacquaniti,et al.  How long did it last? You would better ask a human , 2014, Front. Neurorobot..

[71]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Mind Games: Game Engines as an Architecture for Intuitive Physics , 2017, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[72]  Elise Cournoyer Lemaire The effect of background music on episodic memory. , 2019, Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain.

[73]  Jan Antfolk,et al.  As time passes by: Observed motion-speed and psychological time during video playback , 2017, PloS one.

[74]  Stefan Treue,et al.  The effect of transiency on perceived velocity of visual patterns: a case of “temporal capture” , 1993, Vision Research.