Self-organisation can generate the discontinuities in the somatosensory map

The primary somatosensory cortex contains a topographic map of the body surface, with two notable discontinuities-the representation of the face is next to that of the hands, and that of the feet is next to the genitals. Farah [Why does the somatosensory homunculus have hands next to face and feet next to genitals? a hypothesis. Neural Computation 10(8) (1998) 1983-1985] has suggested that these discontinuities are due to the mechanisms of self-organisation which underlie cortical map development. The typical position of the foetus in the womb means that these two pairs of body parts will often touch and hence their representations will be simultaneously co-active, even though they are distal in terms of the body surface. We use the Kohonen self-organising map algorithm to provide an existence proof of the plausibility of Farah's hypotheses. We then use the model to test the viability of other possible causes of the known map structure and to explore the limitations of self-organisation for explaining the features of the somatosensory map. The model shows that (a) the Kohonen algorithm requires high frequencies of co-activation to introduce a selective discontinuity into the map, and (b) that higher frequency of separate activation of the critical patterns alone is not sufficient to generate the selective discontinuity and (c) the consistency of near-optimal map formation, and in particular the medial-lateral ordering, cannot be reliably generated by a simple Kohonen algorithm.

[1]  Patrick van der Smagt,et al.  Introduction to neural networks , 1995, The Lancet.

[2]  E G Jones,et al.  Hand/Face Border as a Limiting Boundary in the Body Representation in Monkey Somatosensory Cortex , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  Jon H. Kaas,et al.  Central reorganization of sensory pathways following peripheral nerve regeneration in fetal monkeys , 1996, Nature.

[4]  W. Penfield,et al.  The Cerebral Cortex of Man: A Clinical Study of Localization of Function , 1968 .

[5]  J. Elman,et al.  Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development , 1996 .

[6]  Teuvo Kohonen,et al.  Self-organization and associative memory: 3rd edition , 1989 .

[7]  Miguel Á. Carreira-Perpiñán,et al.  Are Visual Cortex Maps Optimized for Coverage? , 2002, Neural Computation.

[8]  J. Kaas,et al.  Variability in hand surface representations in areas 3b and 1 in adult owl and squirrel monkeys , 1987, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[9]  Teuvo Kohonen,et al.  Self-Organization and Associative Memory , 1988 .

[10]  Helmut Laufs,et al.  The Sensory Cortical Representation of the Human Penis: Revisiting Somatotopy in the Male Homunculus , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  Amiram Grinvald,et al.  Visual cortex maps are optimized for uniform coverage , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  Richard Durbin,et al.  A dimension reduction framework for understanding cortical maps , 1990, Nature.

[13]  C. Braun,et al.  Dynamic organization of the somatosensory cortex induced by motor activity. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[14]  Martha J. Farah,et al.  Why Does the Somatosensory Homunculus Have Hands Next to Face and Feet Next to Genitals? A Hypothesis , 1998, Neural Computation.

[15]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Dynamic Plasticity Influences the Emergence of Function in a Simple Cortical Array , 1996, Neural Networks.

[16]  Duane DeSieno,et al.  Adding a conscience to competitive learning , 1988, IEEE 1988 International Conference on Neural Networks.