Opportunity for verbalization does not improve visual change detection performance: A state-trace analysis

Evidence suggests that there is a tendency to verbally recode visually-presented information, and that in some cases verbal recoding can boost memory performance. According to multi-component models of working memory, memory performance is increased because task-relevant information is simultaneously maintained in two codes. The possibility of dual encoding is problematic if the goal is to measure capacity for visual information exclusively. To counteract this possibility, articulatory suppression is frequently used with visual change detection tasks specifically to prevent verbalization of visual stimuli. But is this precaution always necessary? There is little reason to believe that concurrent articulation affects performance in typical visual change detection tasks, suggesting that verbal recoding might not be likely to occur in this paradigm, and if not, precautionary articulatory suppression would not always be necessary. We present evidence confirming that articulatory suppression has no discernible effect on performance in a typical visual change-detection task in which abstract patterns are briefly presented. A comprehensive analysis using both descriptive statistics and Bayesian state-trace analysis revealed no evidence for any complex relationship between articulatory suppression and performance that would be consistent with a verbal recoding explanation. Instead, the evidence favors the simpler explanation that verbal strategies were either not deployed in the task or, if they were, were not effective in improving performance, and thus have no influence on visual working memory as measured during visual change detection. We conclude that in visual change detection experiments in which abstract visual stimuli are briefly presented, pre-cautionary articulatory suppression is unnecessary.

[1]  Andrew R. A. Conway,et al.  On the capacity of attention: Its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive aptitudes , 2005, Cognitive Psychology.

[2]  Andrew Hollingworth,et al.  Binding objects to locations: the relationship between object files and visual working memory. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  Richard D. Morey,et al.  Asymmetric cross-domain interference between two working memory tasks: Implications for models of working memory , 2013 .

[4]  A. Treisman,et al.  Location and binding in visual working memory , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[5]  Nelson Cowan,et al.  When do visual and verbal memories conflict? The importance of working-memory load and retrieval. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[6]  Roy Luria,et al.  Visual Short-term Memory Capacity for Simple and Complex Objects , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  Stanley H. Cohen,et al.  Design and Analysis , 2010 .

[8]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Is the binding of visual features in working memory resource-demanding? , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[9]  Edward K Vogel,et al.  Flexibility in visual working memory: Accurate change detection in the face of irrelevant variations in position , 2012, Visual cognition.

[10]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The design and analysis of state-trace experiments. , 2012, Psychological methods.

[11]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  How to measure working memory capacity in the change detection paradigm , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[12]  Nelson Cowan,et al.  When visual and verbal memories compete: Evidence of cross-domain limits in working memory , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[13]  B. Levy,et al.  Role of articulation in auditory and visual short-term memory , 1971 .

[14]  D. Bamber State-trace analysis: A method of testing simple theories of causation , 1979 .

[15]  Melissa R. Beck,et al.  Attention shifts or volatile representations: What causes binding deficits in visual working memory? , 2012 .

[16]  Edward K Vogel,et al.  Fractionating Working Memory , 2005, Psychological science.

[17]  Richard D. Morey,et al.  A Bayesian hierarchical model for the measurement of working memory capacity , 2011 .

[18]  James R. Brockmole,et al.  Do binding deficits account for age-related decline in visual working memory? , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[19]  G. Woodman,et al.  Storage of features, conjunctions and objects in visual working memory. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  Richard J. Allen,et al.  What you Say Matters: Exploring Visual–Verbal Interactions in Visual Working Memory , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[21]  Yuhong Jiang,et al.  Indirect assessment of visual working memory for simple and complex objects , 2008, Memory & cognition.

[22]  J. Brockmole,et al.  Bound feature combinations in visual short-term memory are fragile but influence long-term learning , 2009 .

[23]  D. J. Murray,et al.  Vocalization-at-presentation and Immediate Recall, with Varying Presentation-Rates , 1965, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[24]  Alan Baddeley,et al.  Disruption of Verbal Stm by Irrelevant Speech, Articulatory Suppression, and Manual Tapping: Do they have a Common Source? , 2003, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[25]  G J Hitch,et al.  Verbal recoding of visual stimuli impairs mentalimagetransformations , 1992, Memory & cognition.

[26]  Denis G. Pelli,et al.  ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.

[27]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[28]  Raymond Bruyer,et al.  Does visual short‐term memory store bound features? , 2004 .

[29]  Louise A. Brown,et al.  Short Article: Limiting the use of Verbal Coding in the Visual Patterns Test , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[30]  J. Schooler,et al.  Verbal overshadowing of visual memories: Some things are better left unsaid , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[31]  R. Morey,et al.  Bayes factors for state-trace analysis , 2016 .

[32]  Andrew Hollingworth,et al.  Does visual short-term memory have a high-capacity stage? , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[33]  G. Sperling,et al.  Successive approximations to a model for short term memory. , 1967, Acta psychologica.

[34]  Nelson Cowan,et al.  How Can Dual-Task Working Memory Retention Limits Be Investigated? , 2007, Psychological science.

[35]  Stephan Lewandowsky,et al.  Rehearsal in serial recall: An unworkable solution to the nonexistent problem of decay. , 2015, Psychological review.

[36]  Louise A. Brown,et al.  Visual working memory is enhanced by mixed strategy use and semantic coding , 2013 .

[37]  G. Woodman,et al.  Selective storage and maintenance of an object’s features in visual working memory , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[38]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[39]  Rachel S. Sussman,et al.  Orienting attention in visual working memory reduces interference from memory probes. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[40]  Jeffrey N Rouder,et al.  An assessment of fixed-capacity models of visual working memory , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[41]  M. D’Esposito Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.