Expected climate change consequences and their role in explaining individual risk judgments

This study examines what individuals expect will be the most important impacts of climate change on their respective countries, and how these expectations relate to individual risk judgments. Open-ended responses from representative samples in four European countries (each n > 1000), were sorted into six categories: expectations of climate change leading to changes in attitudes and goals, human activities, emissions and pollution, environmental changes, impacts on humans, or few or no impacts. The results showed that the most frequently mentioned climate change impacts were related to environmental changes. Although most results were consistent across the UK, Norway, Germany, and France, some differences were identified. For example, respondents in the UK and Norway more frequently mentioned changes in human actions and activities among the most important climate change impacts. We also found differences between demographic groups; men, those in the oldest age groups, and those placing themselves further right on the political spectrum were more likely to expect few or no consequences of climate change on their country. Additional analyses examined relationships between the six impact categories and two different measures of individual risk judgments. Those expecting climate change to lead to changes in attitudes and goals, environmental changes, or impacts on humans reported higher levels of worry about climate change and expected more negative effects on their country. Climate change worry, but not the evaluation of how positive or negative effects will be on one’s country, was further related to the number of consequences mentioned in response to the open-ended question and the specificity conveyed.

[1]  A. Leung,et al.  Perceived cultural impacts of climate change motivate climate action and support for climate policy , 2022, Climatic Change.

[2]  S. Levi Country-level conditions like prosperity, democracy, and regulatory culture predict individual climate change belief , 2021, Communications Earth & Environment.

[3]  G. Bradley,et al.  The nature, significance, and influence of perceived personal experience of climate change , 2020, WIREs Climate Change.

[4]  W. Poortinga,et al.  Political Orientation Moderates the Relationship Between Climate Change Beliefs and Worry About Climate Change , 2020, Frontiers in Psychology.

[5]  Matthew H. Goldberg,et al.  Climate change in the American Mind: April 2020 , 2020 .

[6]  Mike S. Schäfer,et al.  “We are a Bit Blind About it”: A Qualitative Analysis of Climate Change-Related Perceptions and Communication Across South African Communities , 2020 .

[7]  D. Shaw,et al.  Climate change risk perceptions and the problem of scale: evidence from cross-national survey experiments , 2020, Environmental Politics.

[8]  Christina Demski,et al.  Attribution matters: Revisiting the link between extreme weather experience and climate change mitigation responses , 2019, Global Environmental Change.

[9]  A. Leiserowitz Climate Change in the American Mind: National Survey Data on Public Opinion (2008-2017) , 2018 .

[10]  Kim‐Pong Tam,et al.  Environmental Concern Has a Weaker Association with Pro-environmental Behavior in Some Societies than Others: A Cross-Cultural Psychology Perspective , 2017 .

[11]  A. Bostrom Mental Models and Risk Perceptions Related to Climate Change , 2017 .

[12]  Anna Trakoli The Sage Handbook of Risk Communication , 2017 .

[13]  Michael Siegrist,et al.  Knowledge as a driver of public perceptions about climate change reassessed , 2016 .

[14]  Kelly S. Fielding,et al.  Meta-analyses of the determinants and outcomes of belief in climate change , 2016 .

[15]  Tien-ming Lee,et al.  Predictors of public climate change awareness and risk perception around the world , 2015 .

[16]  Kjersti Fløttum,et al.  Explaining topic prevalence in answers to open-ended survey questions about climate change , 2015 .

[17]  A. Burgess The Social Construction of Risk , 2015 .

[18]  Aaron C. Kay,et al.  Solution aversion: On the relation between ideology and motivated disbelief. , 2014, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[19]  F. Rasool,et al.  Cross-Cultural Evidence for Spatial Bias in Beliefs About the Severity of Environmental Problems , 2014 .

[20]  Anthony Leiserowitz,et al.  The Role of Emotion in Global Warming Policy Support and Opposition , 2013, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[21]  Dan M. Kahan,et al.  Ideology, motivated reasoning, and cognitive reflection , 2013, Judgment and Decision Making.

[22]  M. Wildenberg,et al.  Subjective realities of climate change: how mental maps of impacts deliver socially sensible adaptation options , 2013, Sustainability Science.

[23]  A. Spence,et al.  The Psychological Distance of Climate Change , 2012, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[24]  Anthony Leiserowitz,et al.  The Rise of Global Warming Skepticism: Exploring Affective Image Associations in the United States Over Time , 2012, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[25]  M. Siegrist,et al.  Consumers’ knowledge about climate change , 2012, Climatic Change.

[26]  Aaron M. McCright,et al.  Cool dudes: The denial of climate change among conservative white males in the United States , 2011 .

[27]  Montserrat Ochando-Pardo,et al.  Spanish Secondary School Students' Notions on the Causes and Consequences of Climate Change , 2011 .

[28]  L. Whitmarsh What's in a name? Commonalities and differences in public understanding of “climate change” and “global warming” , 2009 .

[29]  A. K. Saysel,et al.  Misperceptions of global climate change: information policies , 2009 .

[30]  J. Sterman Risk Communication on Climate: Mental Models and Mass Balance , 2008, Science.

[31]  A. Leiserowitz Climate Change Risk Perception and Policy Preferences: The Role of Affect, Imagery, and Values , 2006 .

[32]  Sophie A. Nicholson-Cole Representing climate change futures: a critique on the use of images for visual communication , 2005, Comput. Environ. Urban Syst..

[33]  Melissa L. Finucane,et al.  Risk as Analysis and Risk as Feelings: Some Thoughts about Affect, Reason, Risk, and Rationality , 2004, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[34]  H. Pfister,et al.  Mental representation of global environmental risks , 2001 .

[35]  Christopher K. Hsee,et al.  Risk as Feelings , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[36]  Jason C. Kinnell,et al.  Expert and Layperson Perceptions of Ecosystem Risk , 2000, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[37]  Willett Kempton,et al.  How the Public Views Climate Change , 1997 .

[38]  M Granger Morgan,et al.  Now What Do People Know About Global Climate Change? Survey Studies of Educated Laypeople , 1994, Risk analysis : an official publication of the Society for Risk Analysis.

[39]  M. G. Morgan,et al.  What Do People Know About Global Climate Change? 1. Mental Models , 1994 .

[40]  M. Douglas,et al.  Risk and Culture: An Essay on the Selection of Technological and Environmental Dangers , 1983 .

[41]  E. Maibach,et al.  Strategic Communication Research to Illuminate and Promote Public Engagement with Climate Change , 2018 .

[42]  A. Corner,et al.  European Perceptions of Climate Change (EPCC): Topline findings of a survey conducted in four European countries in 2016 , 2017 .

[43]  R. Bark,et al.  Flood risk perceptions and the UK media: Moving beyond “once in a lifetime” to “Be Prepared” reporting , 2017 .

[44]  Hans Spada,et al.  Environmental Risks: Perception, Evaluation and Management , 2001 .

[45]  Wilfried Bos,et al.  Content analysis in empirical social research , 1999 .