Separate Neural Networks for Gains and Losses in Intertemporal Choice

[1]  Lei Zhou,et al.  New Paradigms for the Old Question: Challenging the Expectation Rule Held by Risky Decision-Making Theories , 2018 .

[2]  Marianne C. Reddan,et al.  Modeling Pain Using fMRI: From Regions to Biomarkers , 2018, Neuroscience Bulletin.

[3]  B. Shan,et al.  Distinct Roles of Dopamine Receptors in the Lateral Thalamus in a Rat Model of Decisional Impulsivity , 2017, Neuroscience Bulletin.

[4]  Yuan Zhou,et al.  Gain-loss asymmetry in neural correlates of temporal discounting: An approach-avoidance motivation perspective , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[5]  Takuya Hayashi,et al.  Molecular, Functional, and Structural Imaging of Major Depressive Disorder , 2016, Neuroscience Bulletin.

[6]  A. Rangel,et al.  Activity in dlPFC and its effective connectivity to vmPFC are associated with temporal discounting , 2014, Front. Neurosci..

[7]  Y. Liu,et al.  Resting-State Functional Connectivity Predicts Impulsivity in Economic Decision-Making , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  Jan Peters,et al.  The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal decision-making: understanding variability , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[9]  Jin Fan,et al.  Common and distinct networks underlying reward valence and processing stages: A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies , 2011, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[10]  Niels Birbaumer,et al.  Volitional Control of Anterior Insula Activity Modulates the Response to Aversive Stimuli. A Real-Time Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.

[11]  Eric J. Johnson,et al.  Lateral prefrontal cortex and self-control in intertemporal choice , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  Yasumasa Okamoto,et al.  Sadness enhances the experience of pain via neural activation in the anterior cingulate cortex and amygdala: An fMRI study , 2010, NeuroImage.

[13]  Suzanne H. Mitchell,et al.  The subjective value of delayed and probabilistic outcomes: Outcome size matters for gains but not for losses , 2010, Behavioural Processes.

[14]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Evidence for a Common Representation of Decision Values for Dissimilar Goods in Human Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system , 2009, NeuroImage.

[16]  D. Kumaran,et al.  The Neurobiology of Reference-Dependent Value Computation , 2009, NeuroImage.

[17]  Shu Li,et al.  Neural mechanism of intertemporal choice: From discounting future gains to future losses , 2009, Brain Research.

[18]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Neural Dissociation of Delay and Uncertainty in Intertemporal Choice , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Prefrontal Coding of Temporally Discounted Values during Intertemporal Choice , 2008, Neuron.

[20]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Dissociating the Role of the Orbitofrontal Cortex and the Striatum in the Computation of Goal Values and Prediction Errors , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  C. Pennartz,et al.  Is a bird in the hand worth two in the future? The neuroeconomics of intertemporal decision-making , 2008, Progress in Neurobiology.

[22]  Daniel Read,et al.  Chapter 21. Intertemporal Choice , 2008 .

[23]  P. Glimcher,et al.  The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[24]  G. Berns,et al.  Intertemporal choice – toward an integrative framework , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Orbitofrontal Cortex Encodes Willingness to Pay in Everyday Economic Transactions , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[27]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Time Discounting for Primary Rewards , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  Christopher J Rennie,et al.  Mode of Functional Connectivity in Amygdala Pathways Dissociates Level of Awareness for Signals of Fear , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  M. Walton,et al.  Separate neural pathways process different decision costs , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  Daniel D. Holt,et al.  Differential effects of amount on temporal and probability discounting of gains and losses , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[31]  G. Pagnoni,et al.  Neurobiological Substrates of Dread , 2006, Science.

[32]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Neuroeconomics: cross-currents in research on decision-making , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[33]  Camelia M. Kuhnen,et al.  The Neural Basis of Financial Risk Taking , 2005, Neuron.

[34]  Jeffrey C. Cooper,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of reward prediction , 2005, Current opinion in neurology.

[35]  T. Robbins,et al.  Lesions to the subthalamic nucleus decrease impulsive choice but impair autoshaping in rats: the importance of the basal ganglia in Pavlovian conditioning and impulse control , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[36]  Andrew H. Kemp,et al.  Pathways for fear perception: modulation of amygdala activity by thalamo-cortical systems , 2005, NeuroImage.

[37]  Matthew T. Kaufman,et al.  Distributed Neural Representation of Expected Value , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: insights from neuroimaging , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[39]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Separate Neural Systems Value Immediate and Delayed Monetary Rewards , 2004, Science.

[40]  Saori C. Tanaka,et al.  Prediction of immediate and future rewards differentially recruits cortico-basal ganglia loops , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[41]  A. Sanfey,et al.  Independent Coding of Reward Magnitude and Valence in the Human Brain , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Comparing dynamic causal models , 2004, NeuroImage.

[43]  T. Robbins,et al.  Contrasting Roles of Basolateral Amygdala and Orbitofrontal Cortex in Impulsive Choice , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Does Rejection Hurt? An fMRI Study of Social Exclusion , 2003, Science.

[45]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Both of Us Disgusted in My Insula The Common Neural Basis of Seeing and Feeling Disgust , 2003, Neuron.

[46]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Dissociating Valence of Outcome from Behavioral Control in Human Orbital and Ventral Prefrontal Cortices , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic causal modelling , 2003, NeuroImage.

[48]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The Neural Basis of Economic Decision-Making in the Ultimatum Game , 2003, Science.

[49]  R. Elliott,et al.  Differential Response Patterns in the Striatum and Orbitofrontal Cortex to Financial Reward in Humans: A Parametric Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  J. Deakin,et al.  Effects of quinolinic acid-induced lesions of the orbital prefrontal cortex on inter-temporal choice: a quantitative analysis , 2002, Psychopharmacology.

[51]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Time Discounting and Time Preference: A Critical Review , 2002 .

[52]  Gregory G. Brown,et al.  Error Rate and Outcome Predictability Affect Neural Activation in Prefrontal Cortex and Anterior Cingulate during Decision-Making , 2002, NeuroImage.

[53]  Adrian R. Willoughby,et al.  The Medial Frontal Cortex and the Rapid Processing of Monetary Gains and Losses , 2002, Science.

[54]  J. Deakin,et al.  Effects of lesions of the orbitofrontal cortex on sensitivity to delayed and probabilistic reinforcement , 2002, Psychopharmacology.

[55]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Functional Imaging of Neural Responses to Expectancy and Experience of Monetary Gains and Losses , 2001, Neuron.

[56]  R. Peyron,et al.  Functional imaging of brain responses to pain. A review and meta-analysis (2000) , 2000, Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology.

[57]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Psychophysiological and Modulatory Interactions in Neuroimaging , 1997, NeuroImage.

[58]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Improved Assessment of Significant Activation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI): Use of a Cluster‐Size Threshold , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[59]  M. Zuckerman,et al.  A comparison of three structural models for personality: the big three , 1993 .

[60]  L. Burns,et al.  Time Preference for Health Gains Versus Health Losses , 1993, PharmacoEconomics.

[61]  R. Thaler,et al.  Anomalies: Intertemporal Choice , 1989 .

[62]  G. Loewenstein Anticipation and the Valuation of Delayed Consumption , 1987 .

[63]  G. Ainslie Specious reward: a behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. , 1975, Psychological bulletin.

[64]  P. Samuelson A Note on Measurement of Utility , 1937 .

[65]  B. Gold,et al.  Functional Dissociation in Frontal and Striatal Areas for Processing of Positive and Negative Reward Information , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[66]  G. Ainslie Reasoning: Breakdown of Will , 2008 .

[67]  T. Robbins,et al.  Double dissociation between serotonergic and dopaminergic modulation of medial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex during a test of impulsive choice. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[68]  Nigel Harvey,et al.  Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making , 2004 .

[69]  E. Rolls,et al.  Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[70]  R. Vuchinich,et al.  DELAYED REWARD AND COST DISCOUNTING , 2001 .

[71]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[72]  Wei Wang,et al.  Test of sensation seeking in a Chinese sample , 2000 .

[73]  R. Thaler Some empirical evidence on dynamic inconsistency , 1981 .

[74]  A. Marshall,et al.  Principles of Economics , 1890 .