redundant reaching movements Mechanisms of responsibility assignment during

[1]  R. Sainburg Evidence for a dynamic-dominance hypothesis of handedness , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  N. A. Bernshteĭn The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1967 .

[3]  J I Todor,et al.  Accommodation to increased accuracy demands by the right and left hands. , 1985, Journal of motor behavior.

[4]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Making smooth moves , 2022 .

[5]  Sae Franklin,et al.  Visuomotor feedback gains upregulate during the learning of novel dynamics , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Estimating the sources of motor errors for adaptation and generalization , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[7]  J. Vercher,et al.  Online control of the direction of rapid reaching movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  J. Vercher,et al.  Target and hand position information in the online control of goal-directed arm movements , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Goal-Directed Aiming: Correcting a Force-Specification Error With the Right and Left Hands. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[10]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Specificity of Reflex Adaptation for Task-Relevant Variability , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  Yasmin L. Hashambhoy,et al.  Neural Correlates of Reach Errors , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  Richard G. Carson,et al.  Manual Asymmetries in the Preparation and Control of Goal-Directed Movements , 2001, Brain and Cognition.

[15]  D. Goble,et al.  Task-dependent asymmetries in the utilization of proprioceptive feedback for goal-directed movement , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  Vincent S. Huang,et al.  Rethinking Motor Learning and Savings in Adaptation Paradigms: Model-Free Memory for Successful Actions Combines with Internal Models , 2011, Neuron.

[17]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.

[18]  R. Ivry,et al.  The coordination of movement: optimal feedback control and beyond , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  M. Levin,et al.  Compensatory strategies for reaching in stroke. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  Emanuel Todorov,et al.  Stochastic Optimal Control and Estimation Methods Adapted to the Noise Characteristics of the Sensorimotor System , 2005, Neural Computation.

[21]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Computational nature of human adaptive control during learning of reaching movements in force fields , 1999, Biological Cybernetics.

[22]  R. Sainburg,et al.  Differences in control of limb dynamics during dominant and nondominant arm reaching. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  Etienne Burdet,et al.  Dissociating Variability and Effort as Determinants of Coordination , 2009, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[24]  J. A. Pruszynski,et al.  Long-Latency Reflexes of the Human Arm Reflect an Internal Model of Limb Dynamics , 2008, Current Biology.

[25]  Robert L Sainburg,et al.  Hemispheric specialization for movement control produces dissociable differences in online corrections after stroke. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  J. Diedrichsen,et al.  Responsibility Assignment in Redundant Systems , 2010, Current Biology.

[27]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.