An Infomax Algorithm Can Perform Both Familiarity Discrimination and Feature Extraction in a Single Network

Psychological experiments have shown that the capacity of the brain for discriminating visual stimuli as novel or familiar is almost limitless. Neurobiological studies have established that the perirhinal cortex is critically involved in both familiarity discrimination and feature extraction. However, opinion is divided as to whether these two processes are performed by the same neurons. Previously proposed models have been unable to simultaneously extract features and discriminate familiarity for large numbers of stimuli. We show that a well-known model of visual feature extraction, Infomax, can simultaneously perform familiarity discrimination and feature extraction efficiently. This model has a significantly larger capacity than previously proposed models combining these two processes, particularly when correlation exists between inputs, as is the case in the perirhinal cortex. Furthermore, we show that once the model fully extracts features, its ability to perform familiarity discrimination increases markedly.

[1]  M. W. Brown,et al.  Neuronal activity related to visual recognition memory: long-term memory and the encoding of recency and familiarity information in the primate anterior and medial inferior temporal and rhinal cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  Andrzej Cichocki,et al.  A New Learning Algorithm for Blind Signal Separation , 1995, NIPS.

[3]  Lisa M Saksida,et al.  The Perceptual-Mnemonic/Feature Conjunction Model of Perirhinal Cortex Function , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[4]  Rafal Bogacz,et al.  Model of Familiarity Discrimination in the Perirhinal Cortex , 2004, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[5]  J. Aggleton,et al.  What pharmacological interventions indicate concerning the role of the perirhinal cortex in recognition memory , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  Rosemary A. Cowell,et al.  Why Does Brain Damage Impair Memory? A Connectionist Model of Object Recognition Memory in Perirhinal Cortex , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  Rafal Bogacz,et al.  Comparison of computational models of familiarity discrimination in the perirhinal cortex , 2003, Hippocampus.

[8]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  An Information-Maximization Approach to Blind Separation and Blind Deconvolution , 1995, Neural Computation.

[9]  M. W. Brown,et al.  Evidence concerning how neurons of the perirhinal cortex may effect familiarity discrimination. , 2002, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[10]  Rafal Bogacz,et al.  Model of co-operation between recency, familiarity and novelty neurons in the perirhinal cortex , 2001, Neurocomputing.

[11]  Rafal Bogacz,et al.  Computational models can replicate the capacity of human recognition memory , 2008, Network.

[12]  Teuvo Kohonen,et al.  Self-organization and associative memory: 3rd edition , 1989 .

[13]  Andrew Philippides,et al.  A neural network based holistic model of ant route navigation , 2012, BMC Neuroscience.

[14]  Rafal Bogacz,et al.  Emergence of Movement Sensitive Neurons' Properties by Learning a Sparse Code for Natural Moving Images , 2000, NIPS.

[15]  R. Desimone,et al.  Clustering of perirhinal neurons with similar properties following visual experience in adult monkeys , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  L. Standing Learning 10000 pictures , 1973 .

[17]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  Modeling hippocampal and neocortical contributions to recognition memory: a complementary-learning-systems approach. , 2003, Psychological review.

[18]  Z. Bashir,et al.  Recognition memory and synaptic plasticity in the perirhinal and prefrontal cortices , 2012, Hippocampus.

[19]  Malcolm W. Brown,et al.  Recognition memory: What are the roles of the perirhinal cortex and hippocampus? , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[20]  J. Ringo,et al.  Investigation of long term recognition and association memory in unit responses from inferotemporal cortex , 1993, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  The “independent components” of natural scenes are edge filters , 1997, Vision Research.

[22]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  A model for experience-dependent changes in the responses of inferotemporal neurons , 2000, Network.

[23]  M. W. Brown,et al.  Neuronal evidence that inferomedial temporal cortex is more important than hippocampus in certain processes underlying recognition memory , 1987, Brain Research.

[24]  Adler J. Perotte,et al.  Methods for reducing interference in the Complementary Learning Systems model: Oscillating inhibition and autonomous memory rehearsal , 2005, Neural Networks.

[25]  Thomas G. Dietterich,et al.  Emergence of Motion-sensitive Neurons Properties by Learning Sparse Code for Natural Moving Images , 2001 .

[26]  M. W. Brown,et al.  Recognition memory: neuronal substrates of the judgement of prior occurrence , 1998, Progress in Neurobiology.

[27]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  The effect of learning on the face selective responses of neurons in the cortex in the superior temporal sulcus of the monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  Teuvo Kohonen,et al.  Self-Organization and Associative Memory , 1988 .

[29]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  Independent Component Analysis Using an Extended Infomax Algorithm for Mixed Subgaussian and Supergaussian Sources , 1999, Neural Computation.

[30]  David J. Field,et al.  Sparse coding with an overcomplete basis set: A strategy employed by V1? , 1997, Vision Research.

[31]  I. Riches,et al.  The effects of visual stimulation and memory on neurons of the hippocampal formation and the neighboring parahippocampal gyrus and inferior temporal cortex of the primate , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[32]  R. Desimone,et al.  The representation of stimulus familiarity in anterior inferior temporal cortex. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  T. Bussey,et al.  Perceptual–mnemonic functions of the perirhinal cortex , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  A. Yonelinas The Nature of Recollection and Familiarity: A Review of 30 Years of Research , 2002 .

[35]  J. Knott The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory , 1951 .

[36]  David J. Field,et al.  Emergence of simple-cell receptive field properties by learning a sparse code for natural images , 1996, Nature.

[37]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[38]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Effects of shape-discrimination training on the selectivity of inferotemporal cells in adult monkeys. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  L. Standing Learning 10,000 pictures. , 1973, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[40]  Christof Koch,et al.  Unsupervised Learning of Individuals and Categories from Images , 2008, Neural Computation.

[41]  M. W. Brown,et al.  Differential neuronal encoding of novelty, familiarity and recency in regions of the anterior temporal lobe , 1998, Neuropharmacology.

[42]  R. Desimone,et al.  Activity of neurons in anterior inferior temporal cortex during a short- term memory task , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.