Task-effector asymmetries in a rhythmic continuation task.

Variability in rhythmic movements has been interpreted as a signature of internal or peripheral noise processes. Grounded in an oscillator interpretation, this study hypothesized that period variability and drift arises from the asymmetry between target period and the limb's intrinsic dynamics. Participants synchronized to 7 target periods, swinging 1 of 3 pendulums in a continuation paradigm; 3 periods were longer, 3 shorter, and 1 identical to the preferred period. Results supported 5 predictions: Drift toward the preferred period was observed that scaled with the asymmetry. Variability was lowest for symmetry conditions and increased with the asymmetry. Variability decreased concomitant with the approach toward the preferred period. Periods exponentially approached the preferred period with positive autocorrelations up to 10 cycles.

[1]  Andras Semjen,et al.  On the Timing Basis of Bimanual Coordination in Discrete and Continuous Tasks , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[2]  John H. Challis,et al.  A Procedure for the Automatic Determination of Filter Cutoff Frequency for the Processing of Biomechanical Data , 1999 .

[3]  S. Keele,et al.  Timing Functions of The Cerebellum , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  H. Haken,et al.  A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[5]  R. B. Ivry,et al.  Reduced Timing Variability in Patients with Unilateral Cerebellar Lesions during Bimanual Movements , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  The detuning factor in the dynamics of interlimb rhythmic coordination , 1995, Biological Cybernetics.

[7]  C. Miall Models of neural timing , 1996 .

[8]  Andreas Daffertshofer,et al.  Effects of noise on the phase dynamics of nonlinear oscillators , 1998 .

[9]  E. Large,et al.  The dynamics of attending: How people track time-varying events. , 1999 .

[10]  Alan M. Wing,et al.  Modeling variability and dependence in timing , 1996 .

[11]  Andreas Daffertshofer,et al.  Considerations regarding a comprehensive model of (poly)rhythmic movements , 2000 .

[12]  G. Schöner Timing, Clocks, and Dynamical Systems , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[13]  W. T. Dempster,et al.  SPACE REQUIREMENTS OF THE SEATED OPERATOR, GEOMETRICAL, KINEMATIC, AND MECHANICAL ASPECTS OF THE BODY WITH SPECIAL REFERENCE TO THE LIMBS , 1955 .

[14]  P A Bandettini,et al.  Relationship between Finger Movement Rate and Functional Magnetic Resonance Signal Change in Human Primary Motor Cortex , 1996, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[15]  Franck Vidal,et al.  The supplementary motor area in motor and sensory timing: evidence from slow brain potential changes , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  A. Wing Effects of type of movement on the temporal precision of response sequences , 1977 .

[17]  R. Ivry,et al.  Perception and production of temporal intervals across a range of durations: evidence for a common timing mechanism. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  D. Sternad,et al.  Sinusoidal Visuomotor Tracking: Intermittent Servo-Control or Coupled Oscillations? , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.

[19]  M. Turvey,et al.  An experimental note on defining frequency competition in intersegmental coordination dynamics. , 1996, Journal of motor behavior.

[20]  R. Church,et al.  Preferred rates of repetitive tapping and categorical time production , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  Dagmar Sternad,et al.  Dynamics of 1:2 Coordination: Generalizing Relative Phase to n:m Rhythms. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[22]  P. Fitts,et al.  The frequency response of skilled subjects in a pursuit tracking task. , 1955, Journal of experimental psychology.

[23]  K M Newell,et al.  Energy expenditure and motor performance relationships in humans learning a motor task. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[24]  M. Turvey,et al.  Coupling dynamics in interlimb coordination. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  E. Holst,et al.  The behavioural physiology of animals and man : the selected papers of Erich von Holst , 1973 .

[26]  J. Binder,et al.  Distributed Neural Systems Underlying the Timing of Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Nicholas G. Hatsopoulos,et al.  Coupling the Neural and Physical Dynamics in Rhythmic Movements , 1996, Neural Computation.

[28]  K. Newell,et al.  Time scales in motor learning and development. , 2001, Psychological review.

[29]  R. W. Pew,et al.  Sine-Wave Tracking Revisited , 1967 .

[30]  M. Turvey,et al.  Interlimb coupling in a simple serial behavior: A task dynamic approach , 1998 .

[31]  D. F. Hoyt,et al.  Gait and the energetics of locomotion in horses , 1981, Nature.

[32]  Luke Windsor,et al.  Rhythm Perception and Production , 2000 .

[33]  S. Keele,et al.  Dissociation of the lateral and medial cerebellum in movement timing and movement execution , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  J. Artieda,et al.  Time, internal clocks, and movement , 1996 .

[35]  P. N. Kugler,et al.  Fluctuations and phase symmetry in coordinated rhythmic movements. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  Charles E. Collyer,et al.  A motor timing experiment implemented using a musical instrument digital interface (MIDI) approach , 1997 .

[37]  W. Warren,et al.  Resonance Tuning in Rhythmic Arm Movements. , 1996, Journal of motor behavior.

[38]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Average phase difference theory and 1∶1 phase entrainment in interlimb coordination , 1992, Biological Cybernetics.

[39]  M. T. Turvey,et al.  ‘Clock’ and ‘motor’ components in absolute coordination of rhythmic movements , 1989, Neuroscience.

[40]  E. Saltzman,et al.  Space-time behavior of single and bimanual rhythmical movements: data and limit cycle model. , 1987 .

[41]  M T Turvey,et al.  On the time allometry of co-ordinated rhythmic movements. , 1988, Journal of theoretical biology.

[42]  A. Wing Voluntary Timing and Brain Function: An Information Processing Approach , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[43]  R. Church,et al.  Categorical time production: Evidence for discrete timing in motor control , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[44]  Michael T. Turvey,et al.  Dynamics of human intersegmental coordination: Theory and research , 1998 .

[45]  A. Kristofferson,et al.  The timing of interresponse intervals , 1973 .

[46]  N. R. Bartlett,et al.  Synchronization of a motor response with an anticipated sensory event. , 1959, Psychological review.

[47]  W. Hall,et al.  The Time-Sense , 1927 .

[48]  G. Madison,et al.  Variability in isochronous tapping: higher order dependencies as a function of intertap interval. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[49]  M. Turvey,et al.  Diffusive, Synaptic, and Synergetic Coupling: An Evaluation Through In-Phase and Antiphase Rhythmic Movements. , 1996, Journal of motor behavior.

[50]  R. Llinás,et al.  Dynamic organization of motor control within the olivocerebellar system , 1995, Nature.

[51]  D. McCabe Motor Behavior and Human Skill: A Multidisciplinary Approach , 1998 .

[52]  Richard B Ivry,et al.  Temporal Control and Coordination: The Multiple Timer Model , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[53]  A. Kristofferson,et al.  Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses , 1973 .

[54]  Jan P. Piek,et al.  Motor Behavior and Human Skill: A Multidisciplinary Approach , 1998 .

[55]  M. Turvey,et al.  Advantages of Rhythmic Movements at Resonance: Minimal Active Degrees of Freedom, Minimal Noise, and Maximal Predictability , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.

[56]  N. A. Bernshteĭn The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1967 .

[57]  G. Uhlenbeck,et al.  On the Theory of the Brownian Motion II , 1945 .

[58]  J. Hamill,et al.  Energetic Cost and Stability during Human Walking at the Preferred Stride Frequency , 1995 .

[59]  A. Daffertshofer,et al.  Modeling Rhythmic Interlimb Coordination: Beyond the Haken–Kelso–Bunz Model , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[60]  N. A. Bernstein Dexterity and Its Development , 1996 .

[61]  E. C. Poulton,et al.  Tracking skill and manual control , 1974 .

[62]  Russell M. Church,et al.  A connectionist model of timing. , 1991 .

[63]  Karl M. Newell,et al.  Force and Timing Variability in Rhythmic Unimanual Tapping , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.