Updating Visual Space during Motion in Depth

Whether we are riding in a car or walking, our internal map of the environment must be continuously updated to maintain spatial constancy. Using a memory eye movement task, we examined whether nonhuman primates can keep track of changes in the distance of nearby objects when moved toward or away from them. We report that memory-guided eye movements take into account the change in distance traveled, illustrating that monkeys can update retinal disparity information in order to reconstruct three-dimensional visual space during motion in depth. This ability was compromised after destruction of the vestibular labyrinths, suggesting that the extraretinal signals needed for updating can arise from vestibular information signaling self-motion through space.

[1]  Aldo Genovesio,et al.  Integration of retinal disparity and fixation-distance related signals toward an egocentric coding of distance in the posterior parietal cortex of primates. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  L M Optican,et al.  Saccade-vergence interactions in humans. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  W. King,et al.  Dynamics and efficacy of saccade-facilitated vergence eye movements in monkeys. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Contribution of the otoliths to the calculation of linear displacement. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  G M Jones,et al.  Vestibular-contingent voluntary saccades based on cognitive estimates of remembered vestibular information. , 1988, Advances in oto-rhino-laryngology.

[6]  R H Wurtz,et al.  Disparity sensitivity of frontal eye field neurons. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  Gian F. Poggio Mechanisms of Stereopsis in Monkey Visual Cortex , 1995 .

[8]  C. Bruce,et al.  Primate frontal eye fields. III. Maintenance of a spatially accurate saccade signal. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. , 1985, Science.

[10]  J R Duhamel,et al.  The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.

[11]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Roles of gravitational cues and efference copy signals in the rotational updating of memory saccades. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  W Pieter Medendorp,et al.  Motion Parallax Is Computed in the Updating of Human Spatial Memory , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  G C DeAngelis,et al.  The physiology of stereopsis. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[14]  Gunnar Blohm,et al.  Interaction between smooth anticipation and saccades during ocular orientation in darkness. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  I Israël,et al.  Vestibular information contributes to update retinotopic maps. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[16]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Saccades can be aimed at the spatial location of targets flashed during pursuit. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Saccadic eye movements to flashed targets , 1976, Vision Research.

[18]  L F Dell'Osso,et al.  Saccades to remembered targets: the effects of smooth pursuit and illusory stimulus motion. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Space and attention in parietal cortex. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.

[20]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Primate memory saccade amplitude after intervened motion depends on target distance. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  W Pieter Medendorp,et al.  Rotational Remapping in Human Spatial Memory during Eye and Head Motion , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  L E Mays,et al.  Saccades are spatially, not retinocentrically, coded. , 1980, Science.

[23]  C L Colby,et al.  The analysis of visual space by the lateral intraparietal area of the monkey: the role of extraretinal signals. , 1993, Progress in brain research.

[24]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Estimation of passive horizontal linear whole-body displacement in humans. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Comparison of cortico-cortical and cortico-collicular signals for the generation of saccadic eye movements. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  J W Gnadt,et al.  Eye movements in depth: What does the monkey's parietal cortex tell the superior colliculus? , 1998, Neuroreport.

[27]  Gunnar Blohm,et al.  Processing of retinal and extraretinal signals for memory-guided saccades during smooth pursuit. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  Lawrence H Snyder,et al.  Spatial memory following shifts of gaze. I. Saccades to memorized world-fixed and gaze-fixed targets. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Dora E. Angelaki,et al.  Head unrestrained horizontal gaze shifts after unilateral labyrinthectomy in the rhesus monkey , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  D E Angelaki,et al.  Primate translational vestibuloocular reflexes. I. High-frequency dynamics and three-dimensional properties during lateral motion. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  Christian Quaia,et al.  The maintenance of spatial accuracy by the perisaccadic remapping of visual receptive fields , 1998, Neural Networks.

[32]  R. Andersen,et al.  Memory related motor planning activity in posterior parietal cortex of macaque , 1988, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  A Berthoz,et al.  Spatial memory of body linear displacement: what is being stored? , 1995, Science.

[34]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Properties of signals that determine the amplitude and direction of saccadic eye movements in monkeys. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  M. Loda,et al.  Coding of smooth eye movements in three-dimensional space by frontal cortex , 2022 .

[36]  D Guitton,et al.  Human head-free gaze saccades to targets flashed before gaze-pursuit are spatially accurate. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  L E Mays,et al.  Neurons in monkey parietal area LIP are tuned for eye-movement parameters in three-dimensional space. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  D. Sparks,et al.  Spatial localization of saccade targets. I. Compensation for stimulation-induced perturbations in eye position. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  D. Guitton,et al.  Compensatory eye and head movements generated by the cat following stimulation-induced perturbations in gaze position , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  D E Angelaki,et al.  Three-dimensional organization of otolith-ocular reflexes in rhesus monkeys. III. Responses To translation. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  K Ohtsuka Properties of memory-guided saccades toward targets flashed during smooth pursuit in human subjects. , 1994, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.