Goals, Methods, and Progress in Neuroeconomics

Neuroeconomics shares the main goals of microeconomics: to understand what causes choices, and the welfare properties of choice. The novel goal is linking mathematical constructs and observable behavior to mechanistic details of neural circuitry. Several complementary methods are used. An initial insight from neuroscience is that distinct systems guide choice: Pavlovian and instrumental conditioning (learning) of state-value and response-value associations, overlearned habits, and model- (or goal-) directed value that requires deliberation. These systems can differ economically from rational choice—for example, habitual choices have low utility and price elasticities, whereas model-directed values are often constructed preferences. Neuroeconomics also provides evidence of situations in which utility maximization either works well (in simple binary choice) or benefits from the introduction of behavioral constructs. Neuroeconomics is well equipped to guide the theory of how choices depend on mental states, such as fear or cognitive load. Examples include extensive studies of risk and time preference, finance, and neural decoding of private information.

[1]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Activity in ventromedial prefrontal cortex co-varies with revealed social preferences: evidence for person-invariant value. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[2]  Antonio Rangel,et al.  Neural Activity Reveals Preferences Without Choices , 2013, American economic journal. Microeconomics.

[3]  Jesse M. Shapiro,et al.  WHO IS 'BEHAVIORAL'? COGNITIVE ABILITY AND ANOMALOUS PREFERENCES. , 2013, Journal of the European Economic Association.

[4]  Pietro Ortoleva,et al.  The price of flexibility: Towards a theory of Thinking Aversion , 2013, J. Econ. Theory.

[5]  Colin Camerer,et al.  fMRI evidence of a hot-cold empathy gap in hypothetical and real aversive choices , 2013, Front. Neurosci..

[6]  Colin F Camerer,et al.  Emotion regulation reduces loss aversion and decreases amygdala responses to losses. , 2013, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[7]  Barton L. Lipman,et al.  Advances in Economics and Econometrics: Temptation , 2013 .

[8]  Aureo de Paula,et al.  Econometric Analysis of Games with Multiple Equilibria , 2012 .

[9]  Alireza Soltani,et al.  A Range-Normalization Model of Context-Dependent Choice: A New Model and Evidence , 2012, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[10]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Response of Dorsomedial Prefrontal Cortex Predicts Altruistic Behavior , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  Mareike M. Menz,et al.  Sleep Deprivation Is Associated with Attenuated Parametric Valuation and Control Signals in the Midbrain during Value-Based Decision Making , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  S. Shimojo,et al.  Neural Mechanisms Underlying Paradoxical Performance for Monetary Incentives Are Driven by Loss Aversion , 2012, Neuron.

[13]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Neural circuits in the brain that are activated when mitigating criminal sentences , 2012, Nature Communications.

[14]  Krishna P. Miyapuram Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years later , 2012, Annals of Neurosciences.

[15]  Colin Camerer,et al.  MEASURED ANXIETY AND CHOICES IN EXPERIMENTAL TIMING GAMES , 2012 .

[16]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Equitable decision making is associated with neural markers of intrinsic value , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[17]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  A mechanism for reducing delay discounting by altering temporal attention. , 2011, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[18]  E. Fehr,et al.  Neuroeconomic Foundations of Economic Choice—Recent Advances , 2011 .

[19]  A. Lo Fear, Greed, and Financial Crises: A Cognitive Neurosciences Perspective , 2011 .

[20]  L. G. Crişan,et al.  Cognitive reappraisal reduces the susceptibility to the framing effect in economic decision making , 2011 .

[21]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Insensitivity to social reputation in autism , 2011, Neuroscience Research.

[22]  Jonathan P. Beauchamp,et al.  Molecular Genetics and Economics , 2011 .

[23]  N. Bolger,et al.  Social effects of oxytocin in humans: context and person matter , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  C. Padoa-Schioppa Neurobiology of economic choice: a good-based model. , 2011, Annual review of neuroscience.

[25]  Peter Fransson,et al.  Limbic Justice—Amygdala Involvement in Immediate Rejection in the Ultimatum Game , 2011, PLoS biology.

[26]  Matthias Sutter,et al.  What is for me is not for you: brain correlates of intertemporal choice for self and other. , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[27]  Paul F. Hill,et al.  Remember the Future: Working Memory Training Decreases Delay Discounting Among Stimulant Addicts , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[28]  Hal E. Hershfield,et al.  Increasing Saving Behavior Through Age-Progressed Renderings of the Future Self , 2011, JMR, Journal of marketing research.

[29]  Antonio Rangel,et al.  Hypothetical and Real Choice Differentially Activate Common Valuation Areas , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  Bradford C. Dickerson,et al.  Amygdala Volume and Social Network Size in Humans , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Handbook of Functional MRI Data Analysis: Index , 2011 .

[32]  J. Kable The cognitive neuroscience toolkit for the neuroeconomist: A functional overview. , 2011, Journal of neuroscience, psychology, and economics.

[33]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Testing Theories of Investor Behavior Using Neural Data , 2011 .

[34]  Paola Manzini,et al.  Moody Choice , 2011, SSRN Electronic Journal.

[35]  E. Ciaramelli,et al.  Myopic Discounting of Future Rewards after Medial Orbitofrontal Damage in Humans , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Kenji Matsumoto,et al.  Neural basis of the undermining effect of monetary reward on intrinsic motivation , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[37]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Pavlovian Processes in Consumer Choice: The Physical Presence of a Good Increases Willingness-to-Pay , 2010 .

[38]  Julian J. Emmanuel,et al.  Metabolic State Alters Economic Decision Making under Risk in Humans , 2010, PloS one.

[39]  Peter N. C. Mohr,et al.  Neural Processing of Risk , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  Eric J. Johnson,et al.  Lateral prefrontal cortex and self-control in intertemporal choice , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[41]  Jan Peters,et al.  Episodic Future Thinking Reduces Reward Delay Discounting through an Enhancement of Prefrontal-Mediotemporal Interactions , 2010, Neuron.

[42]  Joseph T. McGuire,et al.  Prefrontal cortex, cognitive control, and the registration of decision costs , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[43]  Antonio Rangel,et al.  Neural computations associated with goal-directed choice , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[44]  Norihiro Sadato,et al.  Processing of the Incentive for Social Approval in the Ventral Striatum during Charitable Donation , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  Marie Claire Villeval,et al.  Cheating, emotions, and rationality: an experiment on tax evasion , 2010 .

[46]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Neural evidence for inequality-averse social preferences , 2010, Nature.

[47]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Amygdala damage eliminates monetary loss aversion , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[48]  Angela Sirigu,et al.  Promoting social behavior with oxytocin in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[49]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Modulation of decision‐making in a gambling task in older adults with transcranial direct current stimulation , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[50]  E. Fehr,et al.  Prejudice and truth about the effect of testosterone on human bargaining behaviour , 2010, Nature.

[51]  Daniel T. Knoepfle,et al.  Value Computations in Ventral Medial Prefrontal Cortex during Charitable Decision Making Incorporate Input from Regions Involved in Social Cognition , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  S. Quartz,et al.  Exploring the Nature of 'Trader Intuition' , 2010 .

[53]  Scott A. Huettel,et al.  Functional Neuroimaging of Intertemporal Choice Models: A Review , 2010 .

[54]  Jeffrey D. Schall,et al.  Brain Imaging for Legal Thinkers: A Guide for the Perplexed , 2009 .

[55]  C. Büchel,et al.  Overlapping and Distinct Neural Systems Code for Subjective Value during Intertemporal and Risky Decision Making , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[56]  Michael L Platt,et al.  Serotonin shapes risky decision making in monkeys. , 2009, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[57]  George Ainslie,et al.  Behavioral and Neural Evidence of Incentive Bias for Immediate Rewards Relative to Preference-Matched Delayed Rewards , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Using Neural Measures of Economic Value to Solve the Public Goods Free-Rider Problem , 2009, Science.

[59]  Camelia M. Kuhnen,et al.  The Influence of Affect on Beliefs, Preferences and Financial Decisions , 2009 .

[60]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Pinocchio's Pupil: Using Eyetracking and Pupil Dilation to Understand Truth-Telling and Deception in Sender-Receiver Game , 2009 .

[61]  R. Nagel,et al.  Neural correlates of depth of strategic reasoning in medial prefrontal cortex , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[62]  Andrew D. Johnson,et al.  Genome-wide association study of blood pressure and hypertension , 2009, Nature Genetics.

[63]  Aldo Rustichini,et al.  Cognitive skills affect economic preferences, strategic behavior, and job attachment , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[64]  D. Kumaran,et al.  A Genetically Mediated Bias in Decision Making Driven by Failure of Amygdala Control , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[65]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system , 2009, NeuroImage.

[66]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Thinking like a trader selectively reduces individuals' loss aversion , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[67]  Mauricio R. Delgado,et al.  Acute Stress Modulates Risk Taking in Financial Decision Making , 2009, Psychological science.

[68]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Economic Games Quantify Diminished Sense of Guilt in Patients with Damage to the Prefrontal Cortex , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[69]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Neural Response to Reward Anticipation under Risk Is Nonlinear in Probabilities , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[70]  D. Mobbs,et al.  When Your Gain Is My Pain and Your Pain Is My Gain: Neural Correlates of Envy and Schadenfreude , 2009, Science.

[71]  P. Glimcher Choice: Towards a Standard Back-pocket Model , 2009 .

[72]  Erkut Y. Ozbay,et al.  Revealed Attention , 2009 .

[73]  B. Balleine,et al.  Multiple Forms of Value Learning and the Function of Dopamine , 2009 .

[74]  Benedetto De Martino,et al.  Explaining Enhanced Logical Consistency during Decision Making in Autism , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[75]  Laurie R Santos,et al.  Endowment effect in capuchin monkeys , 2008, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[76]  N. Barberis,et al.  Realization Utility , 2008 .

[77]  Noah A. Shamosh,et al.  Delay Discounting and Intelligence: A Meta-Analysis. , 2008 .

[78]  Colin Camerer,et al.  A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[79]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Serotonin Modulates Behavioral Reactions to Unfairness , 2008, Science.

[80]  S. Quartz,et al.  The Right and the Good: Distributive Justice and Neural Encoding of Equity and Efficiency , 2008, Science.

[81]  Marcus R. Munafò,et al.  Serotonin Transporter (5-HTTLPR) Genotype and Amygdala Activation: A Meta-Analysis , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[82]  J. Herbert,et al.  Endogenous steroids and financial risk taking on a London trading floor , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[83]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Anchors, scales and the relative coding of value in the brain , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[84]  M. Platt,et al.  Risky business: the neuroeconomics of decision making under uncertainty , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[85]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  The Sunny Side of Fairness , 2008, Psychological science.

[86]  G Elliott Wimmer,et al.  Nucleus accumbens activation mediates the influence of reward cues on financial risk taking , 2008, Neuroreport.

[87]  Selin A. Malkoc,et al.  Discounting Time and Time Discounting: Subjective Time Perception and Intertemporal Preferences , 2008 .

[88]  K. Ochsner,et al.  The role of social cognition in emotion , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[89]  A. Rustichini Neuroeconomics: Formal models of decision making and cognitive neuroscience , 2008 .

[90]  Adriana Galvan,et al.  The adolescent brain. , 2008, Developmental review : DR.

[91]  J. Andreoni Social Image and the 50-50 Norm: A Theoretical and Experimental Analysis of Audience Effects , 2009 .

[92]  P. Glimcher,et al.  The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[93]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Diminishing Risk-Taking Behavior by Modulating Activity in the Prefrontal Cortex: A Direct Current Stimulation Study , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[94]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Social neuroeconomics: the neural circuitry of social preferences , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[95]  D. Schacter,et al.  Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[96]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[97]  Ulrich Mayr,et al.  Neural Responses to Taxation and Voluntary Giving Reveal Motives for Charitable Donations , 2007, Science.

[98]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Time Discounting for Primary Rewards , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[99]  David C. Colander,et al.  Retrospectives: Edgeworth's Hedonimeter and the Quest to Measure Utility , 2007 .

[100]  Sabrina M. Tom,et al.  The Neural Basis of Loss Aversion in Decision-Making Under Risk , 2007, Science.

[101]  R. Hoskins,et al.  Neural Responses to Taxation and Voluntary Giving Reveal Motives for Charitable Donations , 2007 .

[102]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Diminishing Reciprocal Fairness by Disrupting the Right Prefrontal Cortex , 2006, Science.

[103]  Eliza Congdon,et al.  Neural correlates of epigenesis , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[104]  J. Grafman,et al.  Human fronto–mesolimbic networks guide decisions about charitable donation , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[105]  J. Gläscher,et al.  Dissociable Systems for Gain- and Loss-Related Value Predictions and Errors of Prediction in the Human Brain , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[106]  Ran Spiegler,et al.  On two points of view regarding revealed preferences and behavioral economics , 2006 .

[107]  D. Kumaran,et al.  Frames, Biases, and Rational Decision-Making in the Human Brain , 2006, Science.

[108]  S. Quartz,et al.  Neural Differentiation of Expected Reward and Risk in Human Subcortical Structures , 2006, Neuron.

[109]  Lorena R. R. Gianotti,et al.  Disruption of Right Prefrontal Cortex by Low-Frequency Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Induces Risk-Taking Behavior , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[110]  Peter K. Schott,et al.  How Basic Are Behavioral Biases? Evidence from Capuchin Monkey Trading Behavior , 2006, Journal of Political Economy.

[111]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others , 2006, Nature.

[112]  D. Fudenberg,et al.  A Dual Self Model of Impulse Control , 2004, The American economic review.

[113]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Neural Systems Responding to Degrees of Uncertainty in Human Decision-Making , 2005, Science.

[114]  Faruk Gul,et al.  The Case for Mindless Economics , 2005 .

[115]  Camelia M. Kuhnen,et al.  The Neural Basis of Financial Risk Taking , 2005, Neuron.

[116]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Autism: a window onto the development of the social and the analytic brain. , 2005, Annual review of neuroscience.

[117]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Hot-cold empathy gaps and medical decision making. , 2005, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[118]  U. Fischbacher,et al.  Oxytocin increases trust in humans , 2005, Nature.

[119]  A. Meyer-Lindenberg,et al.  5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[120]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Opposing BOLD responses to reciprocated and unreciprocated altruism in putative reward pathways , 2004, Neuroreport.

[121]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Separate Neural Systems Value Immediate and Delayed Monetary Rewards , 2004, Science.

[122]  D. D. de Quervain,et al.  The Neural Basis of Altruistic Punishment , 2004, Science.

[123]  Bonnie Thurston,et al.  Temptation , 2004 .

[124]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The Neural Basis of Economic Decision-Making in the Ultimatum Game , 2003, Science.

[125]  P. Whitney,et al.  Impulsive decision making and working memory. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[126]  Kathleen D. Vohs,et al.  Willpower, choice, and self-control. , 2003 .

[127]  A. Craig How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[128]  M. Egan,et al.  Serotonin Transporter Genetic Variation and the Response of the Human Amygdala , 2002, Science.

[129]  G. Pagnoni,et al.  A Neural Basis for Social Cooperation , 2002, Neuron.

[130]  Christopher K. Hsee,et al.  Risk as Feelings , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[131]  Brian Wansink,et al.  Mindless Eating: Why We Eat More Than We Think , 2001 .

[132]  E. Bullmore,et al.  The amygdala theory of autism , 2000, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[133]  B. Shiv,et al.  Heart and Mind in Conflict: The Interplay of Affect and Cognition in Consumer Decision Making , 1999 .

[134]  Michael L. Platt,et al.  Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex , 1999, Nature.

[135]  N. Aplin,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL EXPECTED UTILITY THEORY AND ANTICIPATORY FEELINGS * A , 1997 .

[136]  G. Loewenstein Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior , 1996 .

[137]  Casey B. Mulligan,et al.  The Endogenous Determination of Time Preference , 1997 .

[138]  Colin Camerer,et al.  The Disposition Effect in Securities Trading: An Experimental Analysis , 1991 .

[139]  W. Mischel,et al.  Cognitive person variables in the delay of gratification of older children at risk. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[140]  Richard P. McLean,et al.  FULL EXTRACTION OF THE SURPLUS IN BAYESIAN AND DOMINANT STRATEGY AUCTIONS , 1988 .

[141]  R. Thaler,et al.  THE BEHAVIORAL LIFE‐CYCLE HYPOTHESIS , 1988 .

[142]  R. Thaler,et al.  An Economic Theory of Self-Control , 1977, Journal of Political Economy.

[143]  David M. Kreps A REPRESENTATION THEOREM FOR "PREFERENCE FOR FLEXIBILITY" , 1979 .

[144]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .