Effects of altering initial position on movement direction and extent.

The purpose of this study was to examine the relative influence of initial hand location on the direction and extent of planar reaching movements. Subjects performed a horizontal-plane reaching task with the dominant arm supported above a table top by a frictionless air-jet system. A start circle and a target were reflected from a horizontal projection screen onto a horizontally positioned mirror, which blocked the subject's view of the arm. A cursor, representing either actual or virtual finger location, was only displayed between each trial to allow subjects to position the cursor in the start circle. Prior to occasional "probe trials," we changed the start location of the finger relative to the cursor. Subjects reported being unaware of the discrepancy between cursor and finger. Our results indicate that regardless of initial hand location, subjects did not alter the direction of movement. However, movement distance was systematically adjusted in accord with the baseline target position. Thus when the hand start position was perpendicularly displaced relative to the target direction, neither the direction nor the extent of movement varied relative to that of baseline. However, when the hand was displaced along the target direction, either anterior or posterior, movements were made in the same direction as baseline trials but were shortened or lengthened, respectively. This effect was asymmetrical such that movements from anterior displaced positions showed greater distance adjustment than those from posterior displaced positions. Inverse dynamic analysis revealed substantial changes in elbow and shoulder muscle torque strategies for both right/left and anterior/posterior pairs of displacements. In the case of right/left displacements, such changes in muscle torque compensated changes in limb configuration such that movements were made in the same direction and to the same extent as baseline trials. Our results support the hypothesis that movement direction is specified relative to an origin at the current location of the hand. Movement extent, on the other hand, appears to be affected by the workspace learned during baseline movement experience.

[1]  A. Georgopoulos Arm movements in monkeys: behavior and neurophysiology , 1996, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[2]  Slobodan Jaric,et al.  The effects of practice on movement distance and final position reproduction: implications for the equilibrium-point control of movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  S. Jaric,et al.  Effects of practice on final position reproduction , 1992, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  M. Kawato,et al.  Trajectory formation of arm movement by cascade neural network model based on minimum torque-change criterion , 1990, Biological Cybernetics.

[5]  M. Kawato,et al.  Coordinates transformation and learning control for visually-guided voluntary movement with iteration: A Newton-like method in a function space , 1988, Biological Cybernetics.

[6]  C. Ghez,et al.  Trajectory control in targeted force impulses , 1987, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Cortical mechanisms related to the direction of two-dimensional arm movements: relations in parietal area 5 and comparison with motor cortex , 1983, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  J. T. Massey,et al.  Interruption of motor cortical discharge subserving aimed arm movements , 1983, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  O. Bock,et al.  Error accumulation and error correction in sequential pointing movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  C Ghez,et al.  Trajectory control in targeted force impulses , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  O. Bock,et al.  Goal-directed arm movements in absence of visual guidance: evidence for amplitude rather than position control , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Arm trajectory formation in monkeys , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  D. Hoffman,et al.  Direction of action is represented in the ventral premotor cortex , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[14]  S. Scott,et al.  Dissociation between hand motion and population vectors from neural activity in motor cortex , 2022 .

[15]  R G Meulenbroek,et al.  Planning reaching and grasping movements: simulating reduced movement capabilities in spastic hemiparesis. , 2001, Motor control.

[16]  D A Rosenbaum,et al.  Frames of reference in perceptual-motor learning: Evidence from a blind manual positioning task , 2001, Psychological research.

[17]  R. Sainburg Evidence for a dynamic-dominance hypothesis of handedness , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  C Ghez,et al.  Learning of Visuomotor Transformations for Vectorial Planning of Reaching Trajectories , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Covariation of primate dorsal premotor cell activity with direction and amplitude during a memorized-delay reaching task. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Apostolos P Georgopoulos,et al.  Neural aspects of cognitive motor control , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[22]  E. Todorov Direct cortical control of muscle activation in voluntary arm movements: a model , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[24]  John W. Krakauer,et al.  Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  D. Hoffman,et al.  Muscle and movement representations in the primary motor cortex. , 1999, Science.

[26]  R L Sainburg,et al.  Intersegmental dynamics are controlled by sequential anticipatory, error correction, and postural mechanisms. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  David A. Rosenbaum,et al.  Remembered positions: stored locations or stored postures? , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  Recent Tests of the Equilibrium-Point Hypothesis (λ Model) , 1998 .

[29]  M. Kawato,et al.  Adaptive internal model of intrinsic kinematics involved in learning an aiming task. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  Recent tests of the equilibrium-point hypothesis (lambda model). , 1998, Motor control.

[31]  C. Ghez,et al.  Discrete and continuous planning of hand movements and isometric force trajectories , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  A. Georgopoulos Current issues in directional motor control , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.

[33]  J. Gordon,et al.  Learning a visuomotor transformation in a local area of work space produces directional biases in other areas. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  T. Ebner,et al.  Temporal encoding of movement kinematics in the discharge of primate primary motor and premotor neurons. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  R L Sainburg,et al.  Control of limb dynamics in normal subjects and patients without proprioception. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  A. Georgopoulos Population activity in the control of movement. , 1994, International review of neurobiology.

[37]  T. Ebner,et al.  Neuronal specification of direction and distance during reaching movements in the superior precentral premotor area and primary motor cortex of monkeys. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  D A Rosenbaum,et al.  A model for reaching control. , 1993, Acta psychologica.

[39]  K. Kurata,et al.  Premotor cortex of monkeys: set- and movement-related activity reflecting amplitude and direction of wrist movements. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Forward Models: Supervised Learning with a Distal Teacher , 1992, Cogn. Sci..

[41]  J F Soechting,et al.  Moving in three-dimensional space: frames of reference, vectors, and coordinate systems. , 1992, Annual review of neuroscience.

[42]  James Gordon,et al.  Organization of voluntary movement , 1991, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[43]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Control of arm movements in a 2-dimensional pointing task , 1990, Behavioural Brain Research.

[44]  M Latash,et al.  [Hypothesis on the equilibrium point and variability of amplitude, speed and time of single-joint movement]. , 1990, Biofizika.

[45]  John M. Hollerbach,et al.  Fundamentals of motor behavior , 1990 .

[46]  A. Riehle,et al.  Monkey primary motor and premotor cortex: single-cell activity related to prior information about direction and extent of an intended movement. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  F. Zajac Muscle and tendon: properties, models, scaling, and application to biomechanics and motor control. , 1989, Critical reviews in biomedical engineering.

[48]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Primate motor cortex and free arm movements to visual targets in three- dimensional space. I. Relations between single cell discharge and direction of movement , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[49]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Primate motor cortex and free arm movements to visual targets in three- dimensional space. III. Positional gradients and population coding of movement direction from various movement origins , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[50]  J. Kalaska The representation of arm movements in postcentral and parietal cortex. , 1988, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[51]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Neuronal population coding of movement direction. , 1986, Science.

[52]  A. G. Feldman Once More on the Equilibrium-Point Hypothesis (λ Model) for Motor Control , 1986 .

[53]  C. Atkeson,et al.  Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[54]  John F. Kalaska,et al.  Spatial coding of movement: A hypothesis concerning the coding of movement direction by motor cortical populations , 1983 .

[55]  A P Georgopoulos,et al.  On the relations between the direction of two-dimensional arm movements and cell discharge in primate motor cortex , 1982, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[56]  D. Rosenbaum Human movement initiation: specification of arm, direction, and extent. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[57]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Characteristics of motor programs underlying arm movements in monkeys. , 1979, Journal of neurophysiology.

[58]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Processes controlling arm movements in monkeys. , 1978, Science.

[59]  P. D. Cheney,et al.  Corticomotoneuronal connections of precentral cells detected by post-spike averages of EMG activity in behaving monkeys , 1976, Brain Research.

[60]  J. Tanji,et al.  Reflex and intended responses in motor cortex pyramidal tract neurons of monkey. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[61]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[62]  E. Evarts,et al.  Relation of pyramidal tract activity to force exerted during voluntary movement. , 1968, Journal of neurophysiology.

[63]  D. Wilkie The mechanical properties of muscle. , 1956, British medical bulletin.

[64]  B. C. Abbott,et al.  The relation between velocity of shortening and the tension‐length curve of skeletal muscle , 1953, The Journal of physiology.