Consistent Chronostasis Effects across Saccade Categories Imply a Subcortical Efferent Trigger

Saccadic chronostasis refers to the subjective temporal lengthening of the first visual stimulus perceived after an eye movement, and is most commonly experienced as the stopped clock illusion. Other temporal illusions arising in the context of movement (e.g., intentional binding) appear to depend upon the volitional nature of the preceding motor act. Here we assess chronostasis across different saccade types, ranging from highly volitional (self-timed saccades, antisaccades) to highly reflexive (peripherally cued saccades, express saccades). Chronostasis was similar in magnitude across all these conditions, despite wide variations in their neural bases. The illusion must therefore be triggered by a lowest common denominator signal common to all the conditions tested and their respective neural circuits. Specifically, it is suggested that chronostasis is triggered by a low-level signal arising in response to efferent signals generated in the superior colliculus.

[1]  M. Goldberg,et al.  The time course of perisaccadic receptive field shifts in the lateral intraparietal area of the monkey. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  C. Pierrot-Deseilligny,et al.  Role of the prefrontal cortex in the control of express saccades. a transcranial magnetic stimulation study , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  W. Meck,et al.  Neuropsychological mechanisms of interval timing behavior. , 2000, BioEssays : news and reviews in molecular, cellular and developmental biology.

[4]  D. C. Howell Statistical methods for psychology, 3rd ed. , 1992 .

[5]  Vincent Walsh,et al.  Auditory Chronostasis Hanging on the Telephone , 2002, Current Biology.

[6]  Kielan Yarrow,et al.  Manual Chronostasis Tactile Perception Precedes Physical Contact , 2003, Current Biology.

[7]  M. Treisman Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval. Implications for a model of the "internal clock". , 1963, Psychological monographs.

[8]  H. Deubel Visual processing and cognitive factors in the generation of saccadic eye movements , 1995 .

[9]  R. Wurtz,et al.  A Pathway in Primate Brain for Internal Monitoring of Movements , 2002, Science.

[10]  Junying Yuan,et al.  Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex , 2022 .

[11]  J. Wearden,et al.  Why “Sounds Are Judged Longer than Lights”: Application of a Model of the Internal Clock in Humans , 1998, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[12]  Stephen M. Rao,et al.  The evolution of brain activation during temporal processing , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Spatial processing in the monkey frontal eye field. I. Predictive visual responses. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  G. Aschersleben,et al.  The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[15]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The effect of frontal eye field and superior colliculus lesions on saccadic latencies in the rhesus monkey. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions , 1978, Vision Research.

[17]  L. Allan The influence of the scalar timing model on human timing research , 1998, Behavioural Processes.

[18]  A. Fuchs,et al.  The brainstem burst generator for saccadic eye movements , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  René M. Müri,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry in cortical control of memory-guided saccades. A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  W. Meck,et al.  Dissecting the Brain's Internal Clock: How Frontal–Striatal Circuitry Keeps Time and Shifts Attention , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[21]  M. Saslow Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. , 1967, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[22]  C. Scudder,et al.  The microscopic anatomy and physiology of the mammalian saccadic system , 1996, Progress in Neurobiology.

[23]  Ake Hellstrom,et al.  The time-order error and its relatives: Mirrors of cognitive processes in comparing. , 1985 .

[24]  Kae Nakamura,et al.  Updating of the visual representation in monkey striate and extrastriate cortex during saccades , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  B. Libet,et al.  Subjective referral of the timing for a conscious sensory experience: a functional role for the somatosensory specific projection system in man. , 1979, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[26]  John C. Rothwell,et al.  Action, arousal, and subjective time , 2004, Consciousness and Cognition.

[27]  J Gibbon,et al.  A new temporal illusion or the TOE once again? , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Neural mechanisms of timing , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  P. Haggard,et al.  Voluntary action and conscious awareness , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  S. Pockett On Subjective Back-Referral and How Long It Takes to Become Conscious of a Stimulus: A Reinterpretation of Libet's Data , 2002, Consciousness and Cognition.

[31]  B. Fischer,et al.  Human express saccades: extremely short reaction times of goal directed eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  M. Treisman,et al.  The Internal Clock: Evidence for a Temporal Oscillator Underlying Time Perception with Some Estimates of its Characteristic Frequency , 1990, Perception.

[33]  D. Burr,et al.  Changes in visual perception at the time of saccades , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[34]  W F Rosenberger,et al.  A sequential design for psychophysical experiments: an application to estimating timing of sensory events. , 1997, Statistics in medicine.

[35]  P. Haggard,et al.  Spatial consequences of bridging the saccadic gap , 2006, Vision Research.

[36]  B. Libet,et al.  Subjective Referral of the Timing for a Conscious Sensory Experience , 1979 .

[37]  Richard A. Tyrrell,et al.  A rapid technique to assess the resting states of the eyes and other threshold phenomena: The Modified Binary Search (MOBS) , 1988 .

[38]  D L Sparks,et al.  Translation of sensory signals into commands for control of saccadic eye movements: role of primate superior colliculus. , 1986, Physiological reviews.

[39]  Frontal eye field: A cortical salience map , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[40]  F. Vidal,et al.  Activation of the supplementary motor area and of attentional networks during temporal processing , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  D. C. Howell Statistical Methods for Psychology , 1987 .

[42]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Neurons in the monkey superior colliculus predict the visual result of impending saccadic eye movements. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  D P Munoz,et al.  Neuronal Correlates for Preparatory Set Associated with Pro-Saccades and Anti-Saccades in the Primate Frontal Eye Field , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  J R Duhamel,et al.  The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.

[45]  John C. Rothwell,et al.  Illusory perceptions of space and time preserve cross-saccadic perceptual continuity , 2001, Nature.

[46]  W. Meck Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[47]  Albert F. Fuchs,et al.  Investigating the site of human saccadic adaptation with express and targeting saccades , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.