Visually Directed Walking to Briefly Glimpsed Targets is not Biased toward Fixation Location
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J S Tittle,et al. The visual perception of three-dimensional length. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] E A Roy,et al. The codability of kinesthetic location and distance information. , 1972, Acta psychologica.
[3] K. N. Leibovic,et al. BINOCULAR SPACE PERCEPTION , 1972 .
[4] W. Gogel,et al. EQUIDISTANCE TENDENCY AND ITS CONSEQUENCES. , 1965, Psychological bulletin.
[5] T. J. Sharkey,et al. Measuring Attention Using Induced Motion , 1989, Perception.
[6] J. M. Foley,et al. Effects of voluntary eye movement and convergence on the binocular appreciation of depth , 1972 .
[7] W C Gogel,et al. Eye Fixation and Attention as Modifiers of Perceived Distance , 1977, Perceptual and motor skills.
[8] E Brenner,et al. Perception and action are based on the same visual information: distinction between position and velocity. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[9] Atsuki Higashiyama,et al. Variation of curvature in binocular visual space estimated by the triangle method , 1981, Vision Research.
[10] J M Foley,et al. The size-distance relation and intrinsic geometry of visual space: implications for processing. , 1972, Vision research.
[11] H Collewijn,et al. Binocular eye movements and the perception of depth. , 1990, Reviews of oculomotor research.
[12] J. Thomson. Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion? , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[13] J. Philbeck,et al. Is the anisotropy of perceived 3-D shape invariant across scale? , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[14] R. Abrams,et al. Differential use of distance and location information for spatial localization , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.
[15] J T Todd,et al. Distortions of Three-Dimensional Space in the Perceptual Analysis of Motion and Stereo , 1995, Perception.
[16] C. Prablanc,et al. Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.
[17] R. Klatzky,et al. Assessing auditory distance perception using perceptually directed action , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.
[18] M A Goodale,et al. The effects of time and distance on accuracy of target-directed locomotion: does an accurate short-term memory for spatial location exist? , 1988, Journal of motor behavior.
[19] John C. Baird,et al. Quantitative functions for size and distance judgments , 1967 .
[20] R. B. Post,et al. Is There Dissociation of Perceptual and Motor Responses to Figural Illusions? , 1996, Perception.
[21] B Gillam,et al. Size and position are incongruous: Measurements on the Müller-Lyer figure , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.
[22] B. Bridgeman,et al. Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.
[23] J. Loomis,et al. Visual space perception and visually directed action. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[24] W C Gogel,et al. A theory of phenomenal geometry and its applications , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.
[25] A. Mack,et al. The dissociation of position and extent in Müller-Lyer figures , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.
[26] J. Rieser,et al. Visual Perception and the Guidance of Locomotion without Vision to Previously Seen Targets , 1990, Perception.
[27] Ralph Norman Haber,et al. Toward a theory of the perceived spatial layout of scenes , 1985, Comput. Vis. Graph. Image Process..
[28] D. Ballard,et al. Memory Representations in Natural Tasks , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[29] W. Epstein. Stability and constancy in visual perception : mechanisms and processes , 1977 .
[30] M. Goodale,et al. Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand , 1995, Current Biology.
[31] L. Jakobson,et al. Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements , 1994, Neuropsychologia.
[32] A. Patla,et al. Locomotor Estimation of Distance after Visual Scanning by Children and Adults , 1985, Perception.
[33] Walter C. Gogel,et al. A two-process theory of the response to size and distance , 1987, Perception & Psychophysics.
[34] D. Elliott. Continuous visual information may be important after all: a failure to replicate Thomson (1983). , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[35] J M Foley,et al. Binocular distance perception: egocentric distance tasks. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[36] R. Toye,et al. The effect of viewing position on the perceived layout of space , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.
[37] J M Loomis,et al. Visually perceived location is an invariant in the control of action , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[38] Ralph Norman Haber,et al. Visual angle as a determinant of perceived interobject distance , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[39] Yves Rossetti,et al. Implicit Short-Lived Motor Representations of Space in Brain Damaged and Healthy Subjects , 1998, Consciousness and Cognition.
[40] S S Fukusima,et al. Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[41] E R Wist,et al. Spatial and fixation conditions affecting the temporal course of changes in perceived relative distance , 1976, Psychological research.
[42] E. Wist,et al. The growth of the equidistance tendency over time , 1968 .
[43] J. Philbeck,et al. Comparison of two indicators of perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[44] Zijiang J. He,et al. Terrain influences the accurate judgement of distance , 1998, Nature.