Fluid intelligence is supported by the multiple-demand system not the language system

A set of frontoparietal brain regions—the multiple-demand (MD) system1,2—has been linked to fluid intelligence in brain imaging3,4 and in studies of patients with brain damage5–7. For example, the amount of damage to frontal or parietal, but not temporal, cortices predicts fluid intelligence deficit5. However, frontal and parietal lobes are structurally8 and functionally9,10 heterogeneous. They contain domain-general regions that respond across diverse tasks11,12, but also specialized regions that respond selectively during language processing13. Since language may be critical for complex thought14–24 (compare with refs 25,26), intelligence loss following damage to the frontoparietal cortex could have important contributions from damage to language-selective regions. To evaluate the relative contributions of MD versus language-selective regions, we employed large functional magnetic resonance imaging datasets to construct probabilistic maps of the two systems. We used these maps to weigh the volume of lesion (in each of 80 patients) falling within each system. MD-weighted, but not language-weighted, lesion volumes predicted fluid intelligence deficit (with the opposite pattern observed for verbal fluency), indicating that fluid intelligence is specifically tied to the MD system, and undermining claims that language is at the core of complex thought.Using fMRI data from healthy controls, the authors construct probabilistic maps of the multiple-demand and language-selective regions in the brain to classify patient lesions. They find that only multiple-demand-weighted lesion volumes predict deficits in fluid intelligence.

[1]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  Broad domain generality in focal regions of frontal and parietal cortex , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[2]  Kathryn M. McMillan,et al.  N‐back working memory paradigm: A meta‐analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[3]  José Luis Bermúdez,et al.  Thinking Without Words , 2007 .

[4]  John Duncan,et al.  Fluid intelligence loss linked to restricted regions of damage within frontal and parietal cortex , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  R J Wise,et al.  Separate neural subsystems within 'Wernicke's area'. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Ape and Human Cognition , 2010 .

[7]  J. Baldo,et al.  Impaired reasoning and problem-solving in individuals with language impairment due to aphasia or language delay , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[8]  Walter Schneider,et al.  The cognitive control network: Integrated cortical regions with dissociable functions , 2007, NeuroImage.

[9]  W. Browner,et al.  Designing Clinical Research , 2006 .

[10]  D. Premack Human and animal cognition: Continuity and discontinuity , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[11]  P. Carruthers The cognitive functions of language , 2002, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[12]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  A functional dissociation between language and multiple-demand systems revealed in patterns of BOLD signal fluctuations. , 2014, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  Christopher L. Asplund,et al.  Functional Specialization and Flexibility in Human Association Cortex. , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  New method for fMRI investigations of language: defining ROIs functionally in individual subjects. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Wolfram Hinzen,et al.  Narrow syntax and the language of thought , 2013 .

[16]  Evelina Fedorenko,et al.  Reliable individual-level neural markers of high-level language processing: A necessary precursor for relating neural variability to behavioral and genetic variability , 2016, NeuroImage.

[17]  J. Duncan,et al.  Fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  John Duncan,et al.  Coding of Visual, Auditory, Rule, and Response Information in the Brain: 10 Years of Multivoxel Pattern Analysis , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  J. Duncan The Structure of Cognition: Attentional Episodes in Mind and Brain , 2013, Neuron.

[20]  Evelina Fedorenko,et al.  Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences Language and Thought Are Not the Same Thing: Evidence from Neuroimaging and Neurological Patients , 2022 .

[21]  Kimberly L. Ray,et al.  Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions , 2012, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[22]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Common fronto-parietal activity in attention, memory, and consciousness: Shared demands on integration? , 2005, Consciousness and Cognition.

[23]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[24]  M. Vernon Relationship of language to the thinking process. , 1967, Archives of general psychiatry.

[25]  S. Petersen,et al.  A dual-networks architecture of top-down control , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  L. Vygotsky,et al.  Thought and language , 1962 .

[27]  Jonathan D. Power,et al.  Control-related systems in the human brain , 2013, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[28]  Jane S. Paulsen,et al.  Estimation of Premorbid Intelligence: The Word Accentuation Test - Buenos Aires Version , 2000, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[29]  Kristina M. Visscher,et al.  A Core System for the Implementation of Task Sets , 2006, Neuron.

[30]  J. Baldo,et al.  Is relational reasoning dependent on language? A voxel-based lesion symptom mapping study , 2010, Brain and Language.

[31]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[32]  J. Duncan,et al.  Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[33]  J. Duncan The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: mental programs for intelligent behaviour , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  J. Naglieri,et al.  Overt and covert verbal problem-solving strategies: developmental trends in use, awareness, and relations with task performance in children aged 5 to 17. , 2003, Child development.

[35]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  Functional specificity for high-level linguistic processing in the human brain , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[36]  Evelina Fedorenko The role of domain-general cognitive control in language comprehension , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[37]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  Language-Selective and Domain-General Regions Lie Side by Side within Broca’s Area , 2012, Current Biology.

[38]  D. Bickerton Language and Human Behavior , 1996 .

[39]  D Rudrauf,et al.  Distributed neural system for general intelligence revealed by lesion mapping , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[40]  John Duncan,et al.  Executive function and fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[41]  Carl Ludy,et al.  Is problem solving dependent on language? , 2005, Brain and Language.

[42]  D. Osherson,et al.  The boundaries of language and thought in deductive inference , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[43]  P. Thompson,et al.  Neurobiology of intelligence: science and ethics , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[44]  Volker C. Franke Continuity and Discontinuity , 1946, Nature.

[45]  Daniel N. Osherson,et al.  Thought Beyond Language , 2012, Psychological science.

[46]  C. Sherwood,et al.  A natural history of the human mind: tracing evolutionary changes in brain and cognition , 2008, Journal of anatomy.

[47]  C. Chabris,et al.  Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[48]  P. Morosan,et al.  Broca's Region: Novel Organizational Principles and Multiple Receptor Mapping , 2010, PLoS biology.

[49]  S. Fernberger The language and thought of the child. , 1927 .

[50]  Derek C. Penn,et al.  Darwin's mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds , 2008, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[51]  R. Varley Reason without much language , 2014 .

[52]  Wolfram Hinzen,et al.  The philosophical significance of Universal Grammar , 2012 .

[53]  Joshua K. Hartshorne,et al.  When Does Cognitive Functioning Peak? The Asynchronous Rise and Fall of Different Cognitive Abilities Across the Life Span , 2015, Psychological science.