Prior behavior impacts human mimicry of robots

Mimicry, the automatic imitation of gestures, postures, mannerisms, and other motor movements, has been shown to be a critical component of human interaction but needs further exploration in human-robot interaction. Understanding mimicry is important for building better robots, learning about human categorization of robots in social ingroups/outgroups, and understanding social contagion in human-robot interaction. We investigate the extent to which humans will mimic a robot during the task of describing paintings by comparing the time participants put their hands on their hips before and after observing a robot cue that behavior. We observed no significant difference in participants' hands on hips time before and after the robot's cue. However, we did find that some participants performed the behavior more after the robot's cue while others performed it less. Furthermore, the direction of this change was a function of whether or not a participant performed the specified behavior prior to the robot's cue. This was similarly observed both for frequency of behavior performance and for a second behavior (hands behind back). As such, this study informs future research on human-robot mimicry, particularly on the importance of prior behavior during a human-robot interaction. In doing so, this study provides a baseline for further understanding and exploring mimicry in human-robot interaction as well as evidence for a social component in human-robot mimicry.

[1]  T. Chartrand,et al.  You Give Me the Chills , 2012, Psychological science.

[2]  U. Hess,et al.  Modulation of facial reactions to avatar emotional faces by nonconscious competition priming. , 2009, Psychophysiology.

[3]  Vilayanur S. Ramachandran,et al.  EEG evidence for mirror neuron activity during the observation of human and robot actions: Toward an analysis of the human qualities of interactive robots , 2007, Neurocomputing.

[4]  Manfred Tscheligi,et al.  Addressing User Experience and Societal Impact in a User Study with a Humanoid Robot , 2009 .

[5]  R. Vonk,et al.  Mimicry in social interaction: benefits for mimickers, mimickees, and their interaction. , 2010, British journal of psychology.

[6]  L. Miles,et al.  Implicit Behavioral Mimicry: Investigating the Impact of Group Membership , 2006 .

[7]  Idit Shalev,et al.  The substitutability of physical and social warmth in daily life. , 2012, Emotion.

[8]  Stefan Kopp,et al.  Exploring the Alignment Space – Lexical and Gestural Alignment with Real and Virtual Humans , 2015, Front. ICT.

[9]  Chen-Bo Zhong,et al.  Cold and Lonely , 2008, Psychological science.

[10]  Maja Pantic,et al.  Sentiment apprehension in human-robot interaction with NAO , 2015, 2015 International Conference on Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction (ACII).

[11]  Paul Pauli,et al.  Modulation of facial mimicry by attitudes , 2008 .

[12]  Tanya L. Chartrand,et al.  I Am Too Just Like You , 2008, Psychological science.

[13]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[14]  M. Bartlett,et al.  Bridging the Mechanical and the Human Mind: Spontaneous Mimicry of a Physically Present Android , 2014, PloS one.

[15]  M. Drury The effects of shared opinions on nonverbal mimicry , 2006 .

[16]  U. Hess,et al.  The impact of social context on mimicry , 2008, Biological Psychology.

[17]  Nigel Crook,et al.  Influence of Upper Body Pose Mirroring in Human-Robot Interaction , 2015, ICSR.

[18]  Cade McCall,et al.  Mimicking disliked others: Effects of a priori liking on the mimicry-liking link , 2009 .

[19]  T. Chartrand The Role of Conscious Awareness in Consumer Behavior , 2005 .

[20]  Peter Robinson,et al.  When my robot smiles at me Enabling human-robot rapport via real-time head gesture mimicry , 2009 .

[21]  Nicolas Guéguen,et al.  Mimicry and Helping Behavior: An Evaluation of Mimicry on Explicit Helping Request , 2011, The Journal of social psychology.

[22]  Roderick I. Swaab,et al.  Early words that work: When and how virtual linguistic mimicry facilitates negotiation outcomes , 2011 .

[23]  Adam D. Galinsky,et al.  Chameleons bake bigger pies and take bigger pieces: Strategic behavioral mimicry facilitates negotiation outcomes , 2007 .

[24]  Paul Pauli,et al.  Electromyographic responses to static and dynamic avatar emotional facial expressions. , 2006, Psychophysiology.

[25]  J. Nadel,et al.  Human responses to an expressive robot , 2006 .

[26]  L. Tiedens,et al.  Power moves: complementarity in dominant and submissive nonverbal behavior. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[27]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The antecedents and consequences of human behavioral mimicry. , 2013, Annual review of psychology.

[28]  J. Bailenson,et al.  Digital Chameleons , 2005, Psychological science.

[29]  Wendy Ju,et al.  Observer Perception of Dominance and Mirroring Behavior in Human-Robot Relationships , 2015, 2015 10th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI).

[30]  Christopher L. Suhler,et al.  When It’s an Error to Mirror , 2011, Psychological science.

[31]  Jennifer J. Argo,et al.  When Imitation Doesn’t Flatter: The Role of Consumer Distinctiveness in Responses to Mimicry , 2011 .

[32]  T. Chartrand,et al.  Using Nonconscious Behavioral Mimicry to Create Affiliation and Rapport , 2003, Psychological science.

[33]  P. Winkielman,et al.  When the social mirror breaks: deficits in automatic, but not voluntary, mimicry of emotional facial expressions in autism. , 2006, Developmental science.

[34]  Luciano Fadiga,et al.  Does Observation of Postural Imbalance Induce a Postural Reaction? , 2011, PloS one.

[35]  C. Heyes,et al.  Robotic movement elicits automatic imitation. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.