Saccadic Modulation of Neural Responses: Possible Roles in Saccadic Suppression, Enhancement, and Time Compression

Humans use saccadic eye movements to make frequent gaze changes, yet the associated full-field image motion is not perceived. The theory of saccadic suppression has been proposed to account for this phenomenon, but it is not clear whether suppression originates from a retinal signal at saccade onset or from the brain before saccade onset. Perceptually, visual sensitivity is reduced before saccades and enhanced afterward. Over the same time period, the perception of time is compressed and even inverted. We explore the origins and neural basis of these effects by recording from neurons in the dorsal medial superior temporal area (MSTd) of alert macaque monkeys. Neuronal responses to flashed presentations of a textured pattern presented at random times relative to saccades exhibit a stereotypical pattern of modulation. Response amplitudes are strongly suppressed for flashes presented up to 90 ms before saccades. Immediately after the suppression, there is a period of 200–450 ms in which flashes generate enhanced response amplitudes. Our results show that (1) MSTd is not directly suppressed, rather suppression is inherited from earlier visual areas; (2) early suppression of the visual system must be of extra-retinal origin; (3) postsaccadic enhancement of neural activity occurs in MSTd; and (4) the enhanced responses have reduced latencies. As a whole, these observations reveal response properties that could account for perceptual observations relating to presaccadic suppression, postsaccadic enhancement and time compression.

[1]  M. Ibbotson,et al.  Enhanced motion sensitivity follows saccadic suppression in the superior temporal sulcus of the macaque cortex. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  Eric Castet,et al.  Motion perception of saccade-induced retinal translation , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[3]  D. Burr,et al.  Selective depression of motion sensitivity during saccades. , 1982, The Journal of physiology.

[4]  J. V. van Hateren,et al.  Dynamics of adaptation at high luminances: adaptation is faster after luminance decrements than after luminance increments. , 1997, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[5]  M Concetta Morrone,et al.  Saccadic eye movements cause compression of time as well as space , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  B. Krekelberg Saccadic suppression , 2010, Current Biology.

[7]  C. Blakemore,et al.  The site of saccadic suppression , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  M. Ibbotson,et al.  Rapid processing of retinal slip during saccades in macaque area MT. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  R. Reid,et al.  Saccadic Eye Movements Modulate Visual Responses in the Lateral Geniculate Nucleus , 2002, Neuron.

[10]  F. Ostendorf,et al.  Perisaccadic mislocalization without saccadic eye movements , 2006, Neuroscience.

[11]  H. E. Roaf The Vertebrate Eye and its Adaptive Radiation , 1943, Nature.

[12]  D. Burr,et al.  Temporal Impulse Response Functions for Luminance and Colour During Saccades , 1996, Vision Research.

[13]  Yang Yang,et al.  Corollary discharge circuits for saccadic modulation of the pigeon visual system , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[14]  J. Malpeli,et al.  Effects of saccades on the activity of neurons in the cat lateral geniculate nucleus. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  G. Karmos,et al.  Transient cortical excitation at the onset of visual fixation. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Visual response latencies of magnocellular and parvocellular LGN neurons in macaque monkeys , 1999, Visual Neuroscience.

[17]  Robert H. Wurtz,et al.  Influence of the thalamus on spatial visual processing in frontal cortex , 2006, Nature.

[18]  Frank Bremmer,et al.  Neural Correlates of Visual Localization and Perisaccadic Mislocalization , 2003, Neuron.

[19]  E. Castet,et al.  ‘Saccadic suppression’ – no need for an active extra-retinal mechanism , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[20]  David W. Royal,et al.  Correlates of motor planning and postsaccadic fixation in the macaque monkey lateral geniculate nucleus , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  F. Duffy,et al.  Electrophysiological Evidence for Visual Suppression prior to the Onset of a Voluntary Saccadic Eye Movement , 1968, Nature.

[22]  J W Gnadt,et al.  The effects of saccadic eye movements on the activity of geniculate relay neurons in the monkey , 2001, Visual Neuroscience.

[23]  D. Burr,et al.  Selective suppression of the magnocellular visual pathway during saccadic eye movements , 1994, Nature.

[24]  R. Wurtz,et al.  A Pathway in Primate Brain for Internal Monitoring of Movements , 2002, Science.

[25]  Michael R. Ibbotson,et al.  Neural basis of time changes during saccades , 2006, Current Biology.

[26]  S Celebrini,et al.  Microstimulation of extrastriate area MST influences performance on a direction discrimination task. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  M. Morrone,et al.  Extraretinal Control of Saccadic Suppression , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  P. Subramanian Active Vision: The Psychology of Looking and Seeing , 2006 .

[29]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Saccadic Suppression , 2002, Science.

[30]  D. Burr,et al.  Changes in visual perception at the time of saccades , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[31]  Stefan Rotter,et al.  Elimination of response latency variability in neuronal spike trains , 2003, Biological Cybernetics.

[32]  Akihiro Yagi,et al.  Reduction of stimulus visibility compresses apparent time intervals , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.