The ignoring paradox: Cueing distractor features leads first to selection, then to inhibition of to-be-ignored items
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Derrick G. Watson,et al. Fractionating the preview benefit in search: dual-task decomposition of visual marking by timing and modality. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] D. Wegner. Ironic processes of mental control. , 1994, Psychological review.
[3] H Pashler,et al. Do images involuntarily trigger search? A test of Pillsbury’s hypothesis , 1999, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[4] Paul E. Downing,et al. Competition in visual working memory for control of search , 2003 .
[5] John Hodsoll,et al. Effects of colour on preview search: anticipatory and inhibitory biases for colour. , 2004, Spatial vision.
[6] H. Egeth,et al. Searching for conjunctively defined targets. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[7] G. Humphreys,et al. An analysis of the time course of attention in preview search , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.
[8] J. Theeuwes,et al. Cueing the location of a distractor: an inhibitory mechanism of spatial attention? , 2008, Acta psychologica.
[9] Min-Shik Kim,et al. Do the contents of working memory capture attention? Yes, but cognitive control matters. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[10] S. Yantis,et al. Preparatory activity in visual cortex indexes distractor suppression during covert spatial orienting. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.
[11] J. Duncan,et al. Directing attention in the visual field , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.
[12] D. Burr,et al. Functional implications of cross-orientation inhibition of cortical visual cells. I. Neurophysiological evidence , 1982, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.
[13] Satoru Suzuki,et al. Understanding priming of color-singleton search: Roles of attention at encoding and “retrieval” , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[14] G. Humphreys,et al. Filtering items of mass distraction: Top-down biases against distractors are necessary for the feature-based carry-over to occur , 2007, Vision Research.
[15] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[16] Jan Theeuwes,et al. An ERP study of preparatory and inhibitory mechanisms in a cued saccade task , 2006, Brain Research.
[17] H. Müller,et al. Attentional capture by salient color singleton distractors is modulated by top-down dimensional set. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[18] K. Cave,et al. Top-down and bottom-up attentional control: On the nature of interference from a salient distractor , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[19] G. Humphreys,et al. Early, involuntary top-down guidance of attention from working memory. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[20] John Hodsoll,et al. Color grouping in space and time: evidence from negative color-based carryover effects in preview search. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[21] P. Downing,et al. Interactions Between Visual Working Memory and Selective Attention , 2000, Psychological science.
[22] G. Humphreys,et al. Automatic guidance of attention from working memory , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[23] J. Wolfe,et al. Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[24] Veit Stuphorn,et al. Trial-by-trial adjustments of top-down set modulate oculomotor capture , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[25] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features , 1994, Memory & cognition.
[26] Alejandro Lleras,et al. Electrophysiological evidence of feature-based inhibition of focused attention across consecutive trials. , 2008, Psychophysiology.
[27] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[28] B. Green,et al. Color coding in a visual search task. , 1956, Journal of experimental psychology.
[29] K. Cave,et al. Spatial Attention in Visual Search for Features and Feature Conjunctions , 1995 .
[30] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Visual marking: using time in visual selection , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[31] M. Posner,et al. Components of visual orienting , 1984 .
[32] S. Luck,et al. Feature-based attention modulates feedforward visual processing , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.
[33] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[34] Y. Tsal,et al. The attentional white bear phenomenon: the mandatory allocation of attention to expected distractor locations. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[35] Hsuan-Fu Chao. Top-down attentional control for distractor locations: the benefit of precuing distractor locations on target localization and discrimination. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[36] J. Wolfe,et al. Second-order parallel processing: visual search for the odd item in a subset. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[37] Christian N L Olivers,et al. What drives memory-driven attentional capture? The effects of memory type, display type, and search type. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[38] E. Fox. Negative priming from ignored distractors in visual selection: A review , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[39] Fred A. Masterson,et al. Pattern redundancy and detection in very short-term memory , 1972 .
[40] F. C. Volkmann,et al. Time course of visual inhibition during voluntary saccades. , 1968, Journal of the Optical Society of America.
[41] Alejandro Lleras,et al. Intertrial inhibition of focused attention in pop-out search , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.
[42] G. Humphreys,et al. Visual marking: prioritizing selection for new objects by top-down attentional inhibition of old objects. , 1997, Psychological review.
[43] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[44] G. Humphreys,et al. Inhibition and anticipation in visual search: Evidence from effects of color foreknowledge on preview search , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.
[45] P. Jolicoeur,et al. A Solution to the Effect of Sample Size on Outlier Elimination , 1994 .
[46] Brian A. Goolsby,et al. Adaptive modulation of color salience contingent upon global form coding and task relevance , 2005, Vision Research.
[47] G. Woodman,et al. Do the contents of visual working memory automatically influence attentional selection during visual search? , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[48] S. Tipper,et al. Selective Attention and Priming: Inhibitory and Facilitatory Effects of Ignored Primes , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[49] M. Masson,et al. Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[50] P Jolicoeur,et al. The linear separability effect in color visual search: Ruling out the additive color hypothesis , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.
[51] J Theeuwes,et al. Visual marking beside the mark: Prioritizing selection by abrupt onsets , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[52] D. Bouwhuis,et al. Attention and performance X : control of language processes , 1986 .
[53] Jon Driver,et al. Attentional Preparation for a Lateralized Visual Distractor: Behavioral and fMRI Evidence , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.