Artificial Life Meets Anthropology: A Case of Aggression in Primitive Societies

One of the greatest challenges in the modern biological and social sciences has been to understand the evolution of altruistic and cooperative behaviors. General outlines of the answer to this puzzle are currently emerging as a result of developments in the evolutionary theories of multilevel selection, cultural group selection, and strong reciprocity. In spite of the progress in theory there is shortage of studies devoted to the connection of theoretical results to the real social systems. This paper presents the model of cooperation which is based on assumptions of heritable markers, constrained resource, and local interactions. Verification of model’s predictions with the real data on aggression in archaic egalitarian societies has demonstrated that initial modeling assumptions are acceptable as major factors of social evolution for these societies.

[1]  A. Vayda Warfare in Ecological Perspective , 1974 .

[2]  Eric Alden Smith,et al.  Human Territoriality: An Ecological Reassessment , 1978 .

[3]  W. Hamilton,et al.  The evolution of cooperation. , 1984, Science.

[4]  Keith F. Otterbein Killing of Captured Enemies: A Cross‐cultural Study1 , 2000, Current Anthropology.

[5]  M. Ember Statistical Evidence for an Ecological Explanation of Warfare , 1982 .

[6]  Mikhail S. Burtsev,et al.  Tracking the Trajectories of Evolution , 2004, Artificial Life.

[7]  Mikhail Burtsev,et al.  An evolutionary agent-based model of pre-state warfare patterns: cross-cultural tests , 2004 .

[8]  Mike Mesterton-Gibbons,et al.  Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation , 2004 .

[9]  H. Gintis,et al.  The evolution of strong reciprocity: cooperation in heterogeneous populations. , 2004, Theoretical population biology.

[10]  P. Shankman Culture Contact, Cultural Ecology, and Dani Warfare , 1991 .

[11]  Joshua M. Epstein,et al.  Growing Artificial Societies: Social Science from the Bottom Up , 1996 .

[12]  Joseph Henrich,et al.  Cultural Evolution of Human Cooperation , 2002 .

[13]  E. Wilson,et al.  Sociobiology: The New Synthesis , 1975 .

[14]  C. Zahn-Waxler,et al.  Unto Others: The Evolution and Psychology of Unselfish Behavior. , 1999 .

[15]  W. Hamilton Innate social aptitudes of man: an approach from evolutionary genetics , 1975 .

[16]  P. Richerson,et al.  Can Group-Functional Behaviors Evolve by Cultural Group Selection?: An Empirical Test , 1995, Current Anthropology.

[17]  B. Kang A Reconsideration of Population Pressure and Warfare: A Protohistoric Korean Case1 , 2000, Current Anthropology.

[18]  Peter Turchin,et al.  Population Dynamics and Internal Warfare: A Reconsideration * , 2006 .

[19]  P. Richerson,et al.  The Origin and Evolution of Cultures , 2005 .

[20]  Peter Hammerstein,et al.  Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation , 2003 .

[21]  P. Richerson,et al.  Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. , 2004 .

[22]  Martin H. Levinson Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution , 2006 .

[23]  Michael X Cohen,et al.  THE ROLE OF SOCIAL STRUCTURE IN THE MAINTENANCE OF COOPERATIVE REGIMES , 2001 .

[24]  C. Ember,et al.  Resource Unpredictability, Mistrust, and War , 1992 .

[25]  P. D. Nolan Toward an Ecological-Evolutionary Theory of the Incidence of Warfare in Preindustrial Societies* , 2003 .

[26]  C. A. Bishop,et al.  Territoriality Among Human Foragers: Ecological Models and an Application to Four Bushman Groups [and Comments and Reply] , 1983, Current Anthropology.

[27]  C. Ember,et al.  War, Socialization, and Interpersonal Violence , 1994 .

[28]  Stephen H. Lekson War in the Southwest, War in the World , 2002, American Antiquity.

[29]  Joshua M. Epstein,et al.  The Evolution of Social Behavior in the Prehistoric American Southwest , 2003, Artificial Life.

[30]  R. Riolo,et al.  Evolution of cooperation without reciprocity , 2001, Nature.