Answer First: Applying the Heuristic-Analytic Theory of Reasoning to Examine Student Intuitive Thinking in the Context of Physics.

We have applied the heuristic-analytic theory of reasoning to interpret inconsistencies in student reasoning approaches to physics problems. This study was motivated by an emerging body of evidence that suggests that student conceptual and reasoning competence demonstrated on one task often fails to be exhibited on another. Indeed, even after instruction specifically designed to address student conceptual and reasoning difficulties identified by rigorous research, many undergraduate physics students fail to build reasoning chains from fundamental principles even though they possess the required knowledge and skills to do so. Instead, they often rely on a variety of intuitive reasoning strategies. In this study, we developed and employed a methodology that allowed for the disentanglement of student conceptual understanding and reasoning approaches through the use of sequences of related questions. We have shown that the heuristic-analytic theory of reasoning can be used to account for, in a mechanistic fashion, the observed inconsistencies in student responses. In particular, we found that students tended to apply their correct ideas in a selective manner that supported a specific and likely anticipated conclusion while neglecting to employ the same ideas to refute an erroneous intuitive conclusion. The observed reasoning patterns were consistent with the heuristic-analytic theory, according to which reasoners develop a “first-impression” mental model and then construct an argument in support of the answer suggested by this model. We discuss implications for instruction and argue that efforts to improve student metacognition, which serves to regulate the interaction between intuitive and analytical reasoning, is likely to lead to improved student reasoning.

[1]  Edward F. Redish,et al.  Making Sense of How Students Make Sense of Mechanical Waves , 1999, physics/0207092.

[2]  David E. Meltzer,et al.  Resource Letter ALIP-1: Active-Learning Instruction in Physics , 2012 .

[3]  Michael C. Wittmann,et al.  Comparing three methods for teaching Newton's third law , 2007 .

[4]  Mel S. Sabella,et al.  Examining the Effectiveness of Clickers on Promoting Learning by Tracking the Evolution of Student Responses , 2008 .

[5]  Miss A.O. Penney (b) , 1974, The New Yale Book of Quotations.

[6]  Mel S. Sabella,et al.  Evidence of Intuitive and Formal Knowledge in Student Responses: Examples from the Context of Dynamics , 2004 .

[7]  Peter S. Shaffer,et al.  Student ability to apply the concepts of work and energy to extended systems , 2009 .

[8]  A. Heckler The Ubiquitous Patterns of Incorrect Answers to Science Questions : The Role of Automatic , Bottom-up Processes , 2011 .

[9]  Chandralekha Singh When physical intuition fails , 2002 .

[10]  Chandralekha Singh,et al.  Assessing Student Expertise in Introductory Physics with Isomorphic Problems. I. Performance on Nonintuitive Problem Pair from Introductory Physics. , 2008, 1602.06499.

[11]  Lei Bao,et al.  Model Analysis: Representing and Assessing the Dynamics of Student Learning. , 2006 .

[12]  Michael E. Loverude Student Understanding of The First Law of Thermodynamics , 2002 .

[13]  Lillian C. McDermott,et al.  Student understanding of the ideal gas law, Part I: A macroscopic perspective , 2005 .

[14]  Yehudit Judy Dori,et al.  The Relationship Between Metacognition and the Ability to Pose Questions in Chemical Education , 2012 .

[15]  Peter S. Shaffer,et al.  Excellence in Physics Education Award Talk: Development of research-based and research-validated curriculum by the Physics Education Group at the University of Washington , 2008 .

[16]  K. Stanovich,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: Individual Differences in Reasoning: Implications for the Rationality Debate? , 2002 .

[17]  Sahana Murthy,et al.  Using introductory labs to engage students in experimental design , 2006 .

[18]  Andrew Elby Helping physics students learn how to learn , 2001 .

[19]  Chandralekha Singh,et al.  What Do Students Do when Asked to Diagnose Their Mistakes? Does It Help Them? II. A More Typical Quiz Context. , 2012 .

[20]  Andrew Elby,et al.  The impact of epistemology on learning: A case study from introductory physics , 2004 .

[21]  Mila Kryjevskaia,et al.  Assessing the Flexibility of Research-Based Instructional Strategies: Implementing Tutorials in Introductory Physics in the Lecture Environment , 2014 .

[22]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability , 1973 .

[23]  MacKenzie R. Stetzer,et al.  Student understanding of wave behavior at a boundary: The limiting case of reflection at fixed and free ends , 2011 .

[24]  MacKenzie R. Stetzer,et al.  Student understanding of wave behavior at a boundary: The relationships among wavelength, propagation speed, and frequency , 2012 .

[25]  MacKenzie R. Stetzer,et al.  Examining inconsistencies in student reasoning approaches , 2013 .

[26]  V. Sawtelle,et al.  "Like Dissolves Like": Unpacking Student Reasoning About Thermodynamic Heuristics , 2014 .

[27]  E. Redish Oersted Lecture 2013: How should we think about how our students think? , 2013, 1308.3911.

[28]  H. Simon,et al.  What is an “Explanation” of Behavior? , 1992 .

[29]  P. Klaczynski,et al.  A dual-process account of the development of scientific reasoning: The nature and development of metacognitive intercession skills , 2008 .

[30]  Keith Frankish,et al.  Dual-Process and Dual-System Theories of Reasoning , 2010 .

[31]  Lillian C. McDermott,et al.  Student understanding of the work‐energy and impulse‐momentum theorems , 1987 .