Fragile knowledge and conflicting evidence: what effects do contiguity and personal characteristics of museum visitors have on their processing depth?

Until recently, museums mainly communicated well-established knowledge. Current science, however, is characterized by a rapid knowledge increase, so that we often have to deal with fragile and inconsistent knowledge. In order to develop exhibitions that encourage visitors to process information in a differentiated way, museums need to know how visitors deal with conflicting information. Furthermore, museum professionals need information on personal and situational factors that may promote the processing of such information. We tested whether conflict processing is influenced by personal characteristics such as situational interest, epistemological beliefs, tolerance of ambiguity, and self-efficacy on the one hand and by situational conditions such as the spatial arrangement of information on the other hand. In two science museums and one museum of cultural history, text pairs were displayed that described a topic from conflicting perspectives. The spatial distance between the two texts was manipulated. Semi-structured interviews and questionnaires captured the personal characteristics of 323 visitors and the way they dealt with conflicting information. The results showed that a majority of the visitors perceived a conflict, and a large proportion was willing to process the information at a deeper level. Sophisticated epistemological beliefs, a high tolerance of ambiguity, and high self-efficacy were found to promote situational interest in conflicting information. High situational interest, in turn, promoted a tendency to process deeply. Placing text pairs in close proximity had a positive effect on the processing depth in science museums but not in the museum of cultural history. A possible explanation lies in the higher density of additional and potentially interfering information in the science museums compared to the museum of cultural history.

[1]  Øistein Anmarkrud,et al.  Prediction of learning and comprehension when adolescents read multiple texts: the roles of word-level processing, strategic approach, and reading motivation , 2013 .

[2]  The Development of an Instrument for the Measuring Students' Cognitive Conflict Levels. , 1999 .

[3]  W. Montague,et al.  Aptitude, Learning, and Instruction , 1980 .

[4]  J. Tirole,et al.  Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation , 2003 .

[5]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Motivated resistance and openness to persuasion in the presence or absence of prior information. , 1993, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.

[6]  Eleanor Rosch,et al.  Principles of Categorization , 1978 .

[7]  E. Frenkel-Brunswik,et al.  Intolerance of ambiguity as an emotional and perceptual personality variable. , 1949, Psychological issues.

[8]  Patricia M. King,et al.  Reflective Judgment: Theory and Research on the Development of Epistemic Assumptions Through Adulthood , 2004 .

[9]  Ulrich Schiefele,et al.  Situational and individual interest. , 2009 .

[10]  E. Rosch,et al.  Cognition and Categorization , 1980 .

[11]  Tobias Richter,et al.  How Nonexperts Understand Conflicting Information on Social Science Issues , 2013, J. Media Psychol. Theor. Methods Appl..

[12]  C. Fletcher,et al.  Causal reasoning in the comprehension of simple narrative texts , 1988 .

[13]  Richard E. Mayer,et al.  Multimedia Learning , 2001, Visible Learning Guide to Student Achievement.

[14]  Geraldine Clarebout,et al.  Compensation Mechanisms when Interacting with Learning Aids , 2011 .

[15]  Ulrich Schiefele Thematisches Interesse, Variablen des Leseprozesses und Textverstehen , 1990 .

[16]  W. Kintsch,et al.  Strategies of discourse comprehension , 1983 .

[17]  Øistein Anmarkrud,et al.  Task-oriented reading of multiple documents: online comprehension processes and offline products , 2013 .

[18]  S. Hidi,et al.  The Four-Phase Model of Interest Development , 2006 .

[19]  Sue Allen,et al.  Looking for Learning in Visitor Talk: A Methodological Exploration , 2003 .

[20]  Doris Lewalter,et al.  Motivationale Aspekte von schulischen Besuchen in naturwissenschaftlich-technischen Museen , 2009 .

[21]  Holger Horz,et al.  Comprehending conflicting science-related texts: graphs as plausibility cues , 2013 .

[22]  Charles M. Reigeluth,et al.  Instructional Design Theories and Models : An Overview of Their Current Status , 1983 .

[23]  S. Toulmin The uses of argument , 1960 .

[24]  Stella Vosniadou,et al.  Mental Models of the Day/Night Cycle , 1994, Cogn. Sci..

[25]  Jason E. Albrecht,et al.  The role of context in accessing antecedents in text , 1991 .

[26]  William F. Brewer,et al.  An empirical test of a taxonomy of responses to anomalous data in science. , 1998 .

[27]  Jürgen Baumert,et al.  Teachers' Beliefs, Instructional Behaviors, and Students' Engagement in Learning from Texts with Instructional Pictures. , 2011 .

[28]  Zahra Soleimani,et al.  Reading Comprehension Strategies , 2013 .

[29]  Richard A. Duschl,et al.  Philosophy of science, cognitive psychology, and educational theory and practice , 1992 .

[30]  K. Crowley,et al.  Learning conversations in museums , 2002 .

[31]  Suzanne Hidi,et al.  An interest researcher's perspective: The effects of extrinsic and intrinsic factors on motivation. , 2000 .

[32]  M. Bradley,et al.  Coactivation and comprehension: Contribution of text variables to the illusion of knowing , 1984, Memory & cognition.

[33]  Paul J. Silvia,et al.  What Is Interesting , 2006 .

[34]  D. Lewalter Cognitive Strategies for Learning from Static and Dynamic Visuals , 2003 .

[35]  S. Hidi,et al.  Interest, Learning, and the Psychological Processes That Mediate Their Relationship. , 2002 .

[36]  Ola Halldén,et al.  Re-framing the problem of conceptual change , 1994 .

[37]  Stephan Dutke,et al.  Using Diagnostic Text Information to Constrain Situation Models , 2010 .

[38]  Kenneth A. Strike,et al.  A revisionist theory of conceptual change , 1992 .

[39]  P. Johnson-Laird Mental models , 1989 .

[40]  Franz Caspar,et al.  Beurteilerübereinstimmung und Beurteilerreliabilität , 2002 .

[41]  J. Falk,et al.  Learning from Museums: Visitor Experiences and the Making of Meaning , 2000 .

[42]  A. Graesser An introduction to strategic reading comprehension. , 2007 .

[43]  R. Mayer,et al.  Multimedia Learning: Frontmatter , 2001 .

[44]  Elizabeth B. Bernhardt,et al.  Learning and comprehension of text , 1988 .

[45]  R. Mayer,et al.  How Seductive Details Do Their Damage: A Theory of Cognitive Interest in Science Learning , 1998 .

[46]  Walter Kintsch,et al.  Comprehension: A Paradigm for Cognition , 1998 .

[47]  P. Pintrich,et al.  Beyond Cold Conceptual Change: The Role of Motivational Beliefs and Classroom Contextual Factors in the Process of Conceptual Change , 1993 .

[48]  Технология Springer Science+Business Media , 2013 .

[49]  Paul Ginns Integrating information: A meta-analysis of the spatial contiguity and temporal contiguity effects , 2006 .

[50]  E. Stahl,et al.  Epistemological beliefs and self-regulated learning with hypertext , 2008 .

[51]  Allan Wigfield,et al.  Handbook of motivation at school , 2009 .

[52]  Ulrich Schiefele,et al.  Motivation und Lernen mit Texten , 1996 .

[53]  S. Kosslyn Image and Brain: The Resolution of the Imagery Debate , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[54]  Murray Singer,et al.  Detecting Causal Inconsistencies in Scientific Text , 1999 .

[55]  Wolfgang Schnotz,et al.  Colorful Bouquets in Multimedia ­Research: A Closer Look at the Modality Effect , 2011 .

[56]  Ioanna Vekiri,et al.  Changes in epistemological beliefs in elementary science students , 2004 .

[57]  P. Johnson-Laird,et al.  Mental Models: Towards a Cognitive Science of Language, Inference, and Consciousness , 1985 .

[58]  Andreas Krapp,et al.  Structural and Dynamic Aspects of Interest Development: Theoretical Considerations from an Ontogenetic Perspective. , 2002 .

[59]  Arthur C. Graesser,et al.  Narrative Comprehension, Causality, and Coherence : Essays in Honor of Tom Trabasso , 1999 .

[60]  Joshua P. Gutwill Challenging a Common Assumption of Hands-On Exhibits: How Counterintuitive Phenomena Can Undermine Inquiry. , 2008 .

[61]  Douglas J. Hacker,et al.  Metacognition in educational theory and practice. , 1998 .

[62]  Jacquelynne S Eccles,et al.  I like to do it, I'm able, and I know I am: longitudinal couplings between domain-specific achievement, self-concept, and interest. , 2007, Child Development.

[63]  Manfred Stede,et al.  Discourse Processing , 2011, NAACL.

[64]  Donna E. Alvermann,et al.  Comprehension of Counterintuitive Science Text: Effects of Prior Knowledge and Text Structure , 1989 .

[65]  M. Anne Britt,et al.  c. Research challenges in the use of multiple documents , 2011 .

[66]  Wolfgang Schnotz,et al.  Task-dependent construction of mental models as a basis for conceptual change , 1997 .

[67]  B. Hofer,et al.  Dimensionality and Disciplinary Differences in Personal Epistemology. , 2000, Contemporary educational psychology.

[68]  I. Ajzen Nature and operation of attitudes. , 2001, Annual review of psychology.

[69]  R. Mayer,et al.  Cognitive Principles of Multimedia Learning: The Role of Modality and Contiguity , 1999 .

[70]  P. Silvia What is interesting? Exploring the appraisal structure of interest. , 2005, Emotion.

[71]  Wolfgang Schnotz,et al.  How do Different Readers Learn with Different Text Organizations , 1982 .

[72]  Geraldine Clarebout,et al.  The relation between self-regulation and the embedding of support in learning environments , 2010 .

[73]  Arthur C. Graesser,et al.  Essays in Honor of Tom Trabasso , 1999 .

[74]  Jr. A. P. Mac Donald,et al.  Revised Scale for Ambiguity Tolerance: Reliability and Validity , 1970 .