Effectiveness of the “Tezawari” and “Hadazawari” sensory test methods in the evaluation of fine-textured knitted fabrics

“Tezawari” and “Hadazawari” are two sensory test methods used for fabric texture evaluation in Japan. In this study, the effectiveness of the two sensory test methods in coolness and moistness discrimination was investigated. Three types of single jersey fabrics that were distinct from each other in thermal and moisture properties were chosen as the samples. The coolness and moistness differences between the three types of samples were evaluated by 20 university students with both “Tezawari” and “Hadazawari” test methods. The comparative analysis of “Tezawari” and “Hadazawari” test results revealed that the coolness differences between samples were even more detectable when discriminated with the “Hadazawari” test method; the between-sample differences in perceived moistness correlated very well with the between-sample differences in perceived coolness in both “Tezawari” and “Hadazawari” tests. In conclusion, compared with the “Tezawari” test method, the “Hadazawari” test method is more suitable for coolness and moistness discrimination.

[1]  Masako Niwa,et al.  Relationship between the warm/cool feeling of fabric and the subjective evaluation of the quality of ladies' knitted fabrics , 2010 .

[2]  Gi-Hun Yang,et al.  Spatial acuity and summation on the hand: The role of thermal cues in material discrimination , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[3]  M A Heller,et al.  Active and passive tactual recognition of form. , 1983, The Journal of general psychology.

[4]  Yi Li,et al.  Mathematical Simulation of the Perception of Fabric Thermal and Moisture Sensations , 2002 .

[5]  Kenneth O. Johnson,et al.  The roles and functions of cutaneous mechanoreceptors , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[6]  Francis McGlone,et al.  Quantifying the sensory and emotional perception of touch: differences between glabrous and hairy skin , 2014, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[7]  B. V. Holcombe,et al.  Fiber Hygroscopicity and Perceptions of Dampness , 1995 .

[8]  O. Rahman The Influence of Visual and Tactile Inputs on Denim Jeans Evaluation , 2012 .

[9]  J. Wessberg,et al.  Pleasantness of touch in human glabrous and hairy skin: Order effects on affective ratings , 2011, Brain Research.

[10]  Marc O. Ernst,et al.  Active Movement Reduces the Tactile Discrimination Performance , 2012, Immersive Multimodal Interactive Presence.

[11]  Hsin-Ni Ho,et al.  Contribution of thermal cues to material discrimination and localization , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  A. Schwartz,et al.  Further analysis of active and passive touch in pattern discrimination , 1975 .

[13]  D. Kenshalo,et al.  Temporal Course of Thermal Adaptation , 1966, Science.

[14]  Dominique Adolphe,et al.  Sensory and Physiological Issues , 2011 .

[15]  Matthias Ringkamp,et al.  Thermoreceptors and thermosensitive afferents , 2009, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[16]  Sachiko Sukigara,et al.  Evaluation of “Shittori” Characteristic for Fabrics , 2008 .

[17]  Yoji Yamada,et al.  Psychophysical Dimensions of Tactile Perception of Textures , 2013, IEEE Transactions on Haptics.

[18]  S. Lederman The perception of surface roughness by active and passive touch , 1981 .

[19]  Barry G. Green, Rya Zaharchuk Spatial variation in sensitivity as a factor in measurements of spatial summation of warmth and cold , 2001, Somatosensory & motor research.

[20]  Lieva Van Langenhove,et al.  The hand of textiles – definitions, achievements, perspectives – a review , 2012 .

[21]  G. Essick,et al.  Touching and feeling: differences in pleasant touch processing between glabrous and hairy skin in humans , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.

[22]  Jürgen Lorenz,et al.  Thermoreceptive innervation of human glabrous and hairy skin: a contact heat evoked potential analysis , 2005, Pain.

[23]  J. Gibson Observations on active touch. , 1962, Psychological review.

[24]  Mark A. Symmons,et al.  Active vs passive touch: the state of play and the future , 2007, Second Joint EuroHaptics Conference and Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems (WHC'07).

[25]  Lars Arendt-Nielsen,et al.  Spatial summation of thermal sensations depends on skin type and skin sensitivity , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  Savvas Vassiliadis Advances in Modern Woven Fabrics Technology , 2011 .

[27]  George Havenith,et al.  The role of decreasing contact temperatures and skin cooling in the perception of skin wetness , 2013, Neuroscience Letters.

[28]  René M. Rossi,et al.  Objective and subjective evaluation of the human thermal sensation of wet fabrics , 2012 .

[29]  J. C. Stevens,et al.  Temperature sensitivity of the body surface over the life span. , 1998, Somatosensory & motor research.

[30]  Roger L. Barker,et al.  Handle of Weft Knit Fabrics , 1992 .

[31]  Yi Li,et al.  Fiber Hygroscopicity and Perceptions of Dampness , 1995 .

[32]  Yi Li,et al.  Fabric Touch Tester: Integrated evaluation of thermal–mechanical sensory properties of polymeric materials , 2006 .

[33]  S J Lederman,et al.  The effect of skin temperature on the perception of roughness. , 1979, Sensory processes.

[34]  Nilgün Özdil,et al.  Sensorial Comfort of Textile Materials , 2012 .

[35]  G. Havenith,et al.  Thermal and tactile interactions in the perception of local skin wetness at rest and during exercise in thermo-neutral and warm environments , 2014, Neuroscience.

[36]  C. Spence,et al.  Multisensory design: Reaching out to touch the consumer , 2011 .

[37]  Loreta VALATKIEN,et al.  Accuracy and Reliability of Fabric ’ s Hand Subjective Evaluation , 2006 .