Cueing vocabulary during sleep increases theta activity during later recognition testing.

Neural oscillations in the theta band have repeatedly been implicated in successful memory encoding and retrieval. Several recent studies have shown that memory retrieval can be facilitated by reactivating memories during their consolidation during sleep. However, it is still unknown whether reactivation during sleep also enhances subsequent retrieval-related neural oscillations. We have recently demonstrated that foreign vocabulary cues presented during sleep improve later recall of the associated translations. Here, we examined the effect of cueing foreign vocabulary during sleep on oscillatory activity during subsequent recognition testing after sleep. We show that those words that were replayed during sleep after learning (cued words) elicited stronger centroparietal theta activity during recognition as compared to noncued words. The reactivation-induced increase in theta oscillations during later recognition testing might reflect a strengthening of individual memory traces and the integration of the newly learned words into the mental lexicon by cueing during sleep.

[1]  J. Fell,et al.  The role of phase synchronization in memory processes , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[2]  J. Born,et al.  Odor Cues During Slow-Wave Sleep Prompt Declarative Memory Consolidation , 2007, Science.

[3]  Joel L. Voss,et al.  Strengthening Individual Memories by Reactivating Them During Sleep , 2009, Science.

[4]  T. Curran,et al.  Functional role of gamma and theta oscillations in episodic memory , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[5]  J. Born,et al.  About sleep's role in memory. , 2013, Physiological reviews.

[6]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data , 2010, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[7]  K. Paller,et al.  Memory stabilization with targeted reactivation during human slow-wave sleep , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[8]  H. Heinze,et al.  The oscillatory dynamics of recognition memory and its relationship to event-related responses. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Electromagnetic source reconstruction for group studies , 2008, NeuroImage.

[10]  K. Paller,et al.  Upgrading the sleeping brain with targeted memory reactivation , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  W Singer,et al.  Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[12]  J. B. Demb,et al.  Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Semantic Memory Processes in the Frontal Lobes , 1996 .

[13]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Stimulation in Hippocampal Region CA1 in Behaving Rats Yields Long-Term Potentiation when Delivered to the Peak of Theta and Long-Term Depression when Delivered to the Trough , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  M Doppelmayr,et al.  A method for the calculation of induced band power: implications for the significance of brain oscillations. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[15]  J. Desmond,et al.  The role of left prefrontal cortex in language and memory. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  Lidia Suárez,et al.  Observing neighborhood effects without neighbors , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[17]  Sung-Phil Kim,et al.  Modulation of theta phase synchronization in the human electroencephalogram during a recognition memory task , 2012, Neuroreport.

[18]  J. Born,et al.  The memory function of sleep , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[19]  E. Düzel,et al.  Induced theta oscillations mediate large‐scale synchrony with mediotemporal areas during recollection in humans , 2005, Hippocampus.

[20]  Alan A. Wilson,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of encoding in episodic memory: levels of processing effect. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  Tim Curran,et al.  EEG oscillations and recognition memory: Theta correlates of memory retrieval and decision making , 2006, NeuroImage.

[22]  J. Fell,et al.  Memory formation by neuronal synchronization , 2006, Brain Research Reviews.

[23]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[24]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Multiple sparse priors for the M/EEG inverse problem , 2008, NeuroImage.

[25]  Tzyy-Ping Jung,et al.  Extended ICA Removes Artifacts from Electroencephalographic Recordings , 1997, NIPS.

[26]  Mircea Ariel Schoenfeld,et al.  A multivariate, spatiotemporal analysis of electromagnetic time-frequency data of recognition memory , 2003, NeuroImage.

[27]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  An empirical Bayesian solution to the source reconstruction problem in EEG , 2005, NeuroImage.

[28]  Guillén Fernández,et al.  Neural correlates of testing effects in vocabulary learning , 2013, NeuroImage.

[29]  J. Lisman,et al.  Bidirectional synaptic plasticity induced by a single burst during cholinergic theta oscillation in CA1 in vitro , 1995, Neuron.

[30]  Craig J. Brozinsky,et al.  Oscillatory EEG correlates of episodic trace decay. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[31]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection theory: A user's guide, 2nd ed. , 2005 .

[32]  Monika Schönauer,et al.  Strengthening Procedural Memories by Reactivation in Sleep , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  B. Rasch,et al.  Boosting Vocabulary Learning by Verbal Cueing During Sleep. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  James M. McQueen,et al.  Changes in Theta and Beta Oscillations as Signatures of Novel Word Consolidation , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.