Patterns of cognitive dissonance-reducing beliefs among smokers: a longitudinal analysis from the International Tobacco Control (ITC) Four Country Survey

Objective The purpose of this paper is to assess whether smokers adjust their beliefs in a pattern that is consistent with Cognitive Dissonance Theory. This is accomplished by examining the longitudinal pattern of belief change among smokers as their smoking behaviours change. Methods A telephone survey was conducted of nationally representative samples of adult smokers from Canada, the USA, the UK and Australia from the International Tobacco Control Four Country Survey. Smokers were followed across three waves (October 2002 to December 2004), during which they were asked to report on their smoking-related beliefs and their quitting behaviour. Findings Smokers with no history of quitting across the three waves exhibited the highest levels of rationalisations for smoking. When smokers quit smoking, they reported having fewer rationalisations for smoking compared with when they had previously been smoking. However, among those who attempted to quit but then relapsed, there was once again a renewed tendency to rationalise their smoking. This rebound in the use of rationalisations was higher for functional beliefs than for risk-minimising beliefs, as predicted by social psychological theory. Conclusions Smokers are motivated to rationalise their behaviour through the endorsement of more positive beliefs about smoking, and these beliefs change systematically with changes in smoking status. More work is needed to determine if this cognitive dissonance-reducing function has an inhibiting effect on any subsequent intentions to quit.

[1]  G. Fong,et al.  Health knowledge and perception of risks among Chinese smokers and non-smokers: findings from the Wave 1 ITC China Survey , 2010, Tobacco Control.

[2]  M. Zanna,et al.  Do risk-minimizing beliefs about smoking inhibit quitting? Findings from the International Tobacco Control (ITC) Four-Country Survey. , 2009, Preventive medicine.

[3]  M. Zanna,et al.  Regret and rationalization among smokers in Thailand and Malaysia: findings from the International Tobacco Control Southeast Asia Survey. , 2009, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[4]  Ron Borland,et al.  Functional beliefs about smoking and quitting activity among adult smokers in four countries: findings from the International Tobacco Control Four-Country Survey. , 2008, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[5]  Nicholas C. Fernandez,et al.  To Practice What We Preach: The Use of Hypocrisy and Cognitive Dissonance to Motivate Behavior Change , 2008 .

[6]  K. Michael Cummings,et al.  The Cigarette Controversy , 2007, Cancer Epidemiology Biomarkers & Prevention.

[7]  A Hyland,et al.  The conceptual framework of the International Tobacco Control (ITC) Policy Evaluation Project , 2006, Tobacco Control.

[8]  G B Hastings,et al.  Methods of the International Tobacco Control (ITC) Four Country Survey , 2006, Tobacco Control.

[9]  W. Klein,et al.  Unrealistic Optimism in Smokers: Implications for Smoking Myth Endorsement and Self-Protective Motivation , 2006, Journal of health communication.

[10]  A. Duggan,et al.  Understanding Interpersonal Communication Processes Across Health Contexts: Advances in the Last Decade and Challenges for the Next Decade , 2006, Journal of health communication.

[11]  T. McCann,et al.  Undergraduate nursing students' attitudes towards smoking health promotion. , 2005, Nursing & health sciences.

[12]  N D Weinstein,et al.  Smokers’ unrealistic optimism about their risk , 2005, Tobacco Control.

[13]  P. Slovic,et al.  Accuracy and optimism in smokers' beliefs about quitting. , 2004, Nicotine & tobacco research : official journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco.

[14]  S. Chapman,et al.  "Bulletproof skeptics in life's jungle": which self-exempting beliefs about smoking most predict lack of progression towards quitting? , 2004, Preventive medicine.

[15]  A. Lazenbatt,et al.  Cognitive dissonance and undergraduate nursing students' knowledge of, and attitudes about, smoking. , 2004, Journal of advanced nursing.

[16]  F. Gibbons,et al.  Perceptions of smoking risk as a function of smoking status , 1992, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[17]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems: Conclusions from a community-wide sample , 1987, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[18]  Josue P. Keely,et al.  Shape of the relapse curve and long-term abstinence among untreated smokers. , 2004, Addiction.

[19]  T. Jørgensen,et al.  How does individual smoking behaviour among hospital staff influence their knowledge of the health consequences of smoking? , 2003, Scandinavian journal of public health.

[20]  Richard P. DeShon,et al.  Combining effect size estimates in meta-analysis with repeated measures and independent-groups designs. , 2002, Psychological methods.

[21]  K. Cummings,et al.  Failed promises of the cigarette industry and its effect on consumer misperceptions about the health risks of smoking , 2002, Tobacco control.

[22]  N. Weinstein Accuracy of smokers' risk perceptions , 1998, Annals of behavioral medicine : a publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine.

[23]  R. Nisbett,et al.  The cultural matrix of social psychology , 1998 .

[24]  E. Aronson,et al.  When exemplification fails: hypocrisy and the motive for self-integrity. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[25]  Linda Simon,et al.  Trivialization: the forgotten mode of dissonance reduction. , 1995 .

[26]  Matthew P. Winslow,et al.  Inducing Hypocrisy as a Means of Encouraging Young Adults to Use Condoms , 1994 .

[27]  S. Chapman,et al.  Self-exempting beliefs about smoking and health: differences between smokers and ex-smokers. , 1993, American journal of public health.

[28]  Jacob Cohen,et al.  A power primer. , 1992, Psychological bulletin.

[29]  Elliot Aronson,et al.  Using Cognitive Dissonance to Encourage Water Conservation) , 1992 .

[30]  C McMaster,et al.  Cognitive dissonance in tobacco smokers. , 1991, Addictive behaviors.

[31]  Z. Kunda,et al.  The case for motivated reasoning. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.

[32]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Illusion and well-being: a social psychological perspective on mental health. , 1988, Psychological bulletin.

[33]  C. Steele The Psychology of Self-Affirmation: Sustaining the Integrity of the Self , 1988 .

[34]  C. T. Salmon,et al.  The Next-Birthday Method of Respondent Selection , 1983 .

[35]  Myers Ml,et al.  Public version: Federal Trade Commission staff report on the cigarette advertising investigation. , 1981 .

[36]  S H Croog,et al.  Health beliefs and smoking patterns in heart patients and their wives: a longitudinal study. , 1977, American journal of public health.

[37]  J Cooper,et al.  Dissonance and the pill: an attribution approach to studying the arousal properties of dissonance. , 1974, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[38]  R. E. Johnson Smoking and the reduction of cognitive dissonance. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[39]  William J. McGuire,et al.  Resistance to persuasion conferred by active and passive prior refutation of the same and alternative counterarguments. , 1961 .

[40]  L. Festinger,et al.  Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. , 2011, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[41]  L. Festinger,et al.  A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance , 2017 .

[42]  S. Fiske,et al.  The Handbook of Social Psychology , 1935 .