On the misery of cognitive effort

The effect of cognitive effort on mood is unclear. Expending cognitive effort is generally avoided but expenditure of cognitive effort is the primary feature of some popular recreational activities, such as sudoku or video games. However, one common confound is that previous studies mostly looked into mood changes before and after cognitive effort manipulation. Therefore, it is unknown what immediate impact cognitive effort has on momentary mood. To investigate this, in two studies, we used a letter sorting paradigm to test whether momentary mood ratings change with different levels of experimentally manipulated task difficulty. In study 1 (N = 105), we found that increased difficulty, and consequently cognitive effort, leads to more errors and lower mood ratings. In study 2 (N = 210), we replicated our results from study 1 and further demonstrated that cognitive effort influences mood independently from immediate reward. In conclusion, controlling for reward conditions, we found that cognitive effort reduces people’s moment to

[1]  Adam G. Thomas,et al.  A highly replicable decline in mood during rest and simple tasks , 2023, Nature Human Behaviour.

[2]  Eran Eldar,et al.  Emotions as computations , 2022, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[3]  A. Saleem,et al.  Midbrain dopamine neurons signal phasic and ramping reward prediction error during goal-directed navigation , 2022, Cell reports.

[4]  R. Carels,et al.  Mobile assessment of experiential avoidance, mood, stress, and attendance in cardiopulmonary rehabilitation. , 2022, Health Psychology.

[5]  Paul M. Krueger,et al.  Rational use of cognitive resources in human planning , 2022, Nature Human Behaviour.

[6]  D. Nielson,et al.  Magnetoencephalographic correlates of mood and reward dynamics in human adolescents , 2021, Cerebral cortex.

[7]  Y. Niv,et al.  Affect-congruent attention drives changes in reward expectations , 2021 .

[8]  J. Grèzes,et al.  Impact of total sleep deprivation and related mood changes on approach-avoidance decisions to threat-related facial displays , 2021, Sleep.

[9]  D. Nielson,et al.  The temporal representation of experience in subjective mood , 2021, eLife.

[10]  Mark K. Ho,et al.  People construct simplified mental representations to plan , 2021, Nature.

[11]  Yael Niv,et al.  Signed and unsigned reward prediction errors dynamically enhance learning and memory , 2021, eLife.

[12]  Herman Aguinis,et al.  MTurk Research: Review and Recommendations , 2020, Journal of Management.

[13]  N. Steinbeis,et al.  Effort-related decision-making and its underlying processes during childhood. , 2020, Developmental psychology.

[14]  José R. Donoso,et al.  Trial-by-trial dynamics of reward prediction error-associated signals during extinction learning and renewal , 2020, Progress in Neurobiology.

[15]  N. Kroemer,et al.  Non-invasive vagus nerve stimulation boosts mood recovery after effort exertion , 2020, bioRxiv.

[16]  A. R. Otto,et al.  Forced choices reveal a trade-off between cognitive effort and physical pain , 2020, eLife.

[17]  M. Inzlicht,et al.  When effort matters: Expectations of reward and efficacy guide cognitive control allocation , 2020, bioRxiv.

[18]  M. Diefenbach,et al.  The Relationship Between Uncertainty and Affect , 2019, Front. Psychol..

[19]  L. Morey,et al.  Who Are the Turkers? A Characterization of MTurk Workers Using the Personality Assessment Inventory , 2019, Assessment.

[20]  Elissa M. Redmiles,et al.  How Well Do My Results Generalize? Comparing Security and Privacy Survey Results from MTurk, Web, and Telephone Samples , 2019, 2019 IEEE Symposium on Security and Privacy (SP).

[21]  G. O’Callaghan,et al.  Reward Processing in Adolescent Depression Across Neuroimaging Modalities. , 2019, Zeitschrift fur Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie.

[22]  E. Bijleveld The feeling of effort during mental activity , 2018, Consciousness and Cognition.

[23]  S. Massar,et al.  Quantifying the Motivational Effects of Cognitive Fatigue Through Effort-Based Decision Making , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[24]  Peter Dayan,et al.  Decodability of Reward Learning Signals Predicts Mood Fluctuations , 2018, Current Biology.

[25]  Christopher Y. Olivola,et al.  The Effort Paradox: Effort Is Both Costly and Valued , 2018, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  A. R. Otto,et al.  Cognitive capacity limitations and Need for Cognition differentially predict reward-induced cognitive effort expenditure , 2018, Cognition.

[27]  Laura Murray,et al.  Impaired reward prediction error encoding and striatal-midbrain connectivity in depression , 2018, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[28]  Karoline Mortensen,et al.  Comparing Amazon’s Mechanical Turk Platform to Conventional Data Collection Methods in the Health and Medical Research Literature , 2018, Journal of General Internal Medicine.

[29]  P. Dayan,et al.  Attenuation of dopamine-modulated prefrontal value signals underlies probabilistic reward learning deficits in old age , 2017, eLife.

[30]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Toward a Rational and Mechanistic Account of Mental Effort. , 2017, Annual review of neuroscience.

[31]  Jenifer Z. Siegel,et al.  Association of Neural and Emotional Impacts of Reward Prediction Errors With Major Depression , 2017, JAMA psychiatry.

[32]  M. Husain,et al.  Prosocial apathy for helping others when effort is required , 2017, Nature Human Behaviour.

[33]  Alexander W. Johnson,et al.  Disruptions in effort-based decision-making and consummatory behavior following antagonism of the dopamine D2 receptor , 2017, Behavioural Brain Research.

[34]  Lei Wang,et al.  Effort provides its own reward: endeavors reinforce subjective expectation and evaluation of task performance , 2017, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  M. Tomasello,et al.  The Early Emergence of Guilt-Motivated Prosocial Behavior. , 2016, Child development.

[36]  Wouter Kool,et al.  When Does Model-Based Control Pay Off? , 2016, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[37]  N. Daw,et al.  Taking Psychiatry Research Online , 2016, Neuron.

[38]  Y. Niv,et al.  Mood as Representation of Momentum , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[39]  P. Dayan,et al.  Dopaminergic Modulation of Decision Making and Subjective Well-Being , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  D. Tingley,et al.  “Who are these people?” Evaluating the demographic characteristics and political preferences of MTurk survey respondents , 2015 .

[41]  Jia Huang,et al.  Motivational deficits in effort-based decision making in individuals with subsyndromal depression, first-episode and remitted depression patients , 2014, Psychiatry Research.

[42]  H. Berenbaum,et al.  Substantial symptom changes in naturalistic recovery from aversive events. , 2014, Journal of clinical psychology.

[43]  Ezequiel Morsella,et al.  The subjective effort of everyday mental tasks: Attending, assessing, and choosing , 2014 .

[44]  P. Dayan,et al.  A computational and neural model of momentary subjective well-being , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[45]  D. Bates,et al.  Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.

[46]  Michiru Nagatsu,et al.  The Social Motivation Hypothesis for Prosocial Behavior , 2014 .

[47]  T. Braver,et al.  What Is the Subjective Cost of Cognitive Effort? Load, Trait, and Aging Effects Revealed by Economic Preference , 2013, PloS one.

[48]  P. Dayan,et al.  Effort and Valuation in the Brain: The Effects of Anticipation and Execution , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  W. Lawson Autism spectrum conditions: the pathophysiological basis for inattention and the new Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V) , 2013 .

[50]  Jesse Chandler,et al.  Using Mechanical Turk to Study Clinical Populations , 2013 .

[51]  Adam M. Grant,et al.  Doing Good at Work Feels Good at Home, but Not Right Away: When and Why Perceived Prosocial Impact Predicts Positive Affect , 2012 .

[52]  R. Shelton,et al.  Effort-based decision-making in major depressive disorder: a translational model of motivational anhedonia. , 2012, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[53]  Peter Dayan,et al.  Bonsai Trees in Your Head: How the Pavlovian System Sculpts Goal-Directed Choices by Pruning Decision Trees , 2012, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[54]  J. Aw,et al.  How costs affect preferences: experiments on state dependence, hedonic state and within-trial contrast in starlings , 2011, Animal Behaviour.

[55]  P. Dayan,et al.  States versus Rewards: Dissociable Neural Prediction Error Signals Underlying Model-Based and Model-Free Reinforcement Learning , 2010, Neuron.

[56]  Robert Kurzban,et al.  Does the Brain Consume Additional Glucose during Self-Control Tasks? , 2010, Evolutionary psychology : an international journal of evolutionary approaches to psychology and behavior.

[57]  D. Zald,et al.  Worth the ‘EEfRT’? The Effort Expenditure for Rewards Task as an Objective Measure of Motivation and Anhedonia , 2009, PloS one.

[58]  Joseph T. McGuire,et al.  Effort discounting in human nucleus accumbens , 2009, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[59]  J H Duyn,et al.  fMRI study of effort and information processing in a working memory task , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[60]  P. Dayan,et al.  Tonic dopamine: opportunity costs and the control of response vigor , 2007, Psychopharmacology.

[61]  O. Wolf,et al.  Mood changes in response to psychosocial stress in healthy young women: effects of pretreatment with cortisol. , 2007, Behavioral neuroscience.

[62]  G. Gendolla,et al.  The joint effect of informational mood impact and performance-contingent consequences on effort-related cardiovascular response. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[63]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[64]  N. Lassen,et al.  Persistent Resetting of the Cerebral Oxygen/Glucose Uptake Ratio by Brain Activation: Evidence Obtained with the Kety—Schmidt Technique , 1995, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[65]  A. Tesser,et al.  Task effort and the regulation of mood: The absorption hypothesis , 1992 .

[66]  Stuart J. Russell,et al.  Principles of Metareasoning , 1989, Artif. Intell..

[67]  Ed Diener,et al.  Affect intensity and reactions to daily life events. , 1986 .

[68]  N. L. Mintz,et al.  Effects of Esthetic Surroundings: II. Prolonged and Repeated Experience in a “Beautiful” and an “Ugly” Room , 1956 .

[69]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[70]  P. Dayan,et al.  NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neuron. Author manuscript. , 2011 .

[71]  J. D. Ramos Mood and mental effort : informational mood impact on cardiovascular reactivity and the context-dependency of moods , 2009 .

[72]  G. Gendolla,et al.  Does depression interfere with effort mobilization? Effects of dysphoria and task difficulty on cardiovascular response. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[73]  Richard E. Lucas,et al.  Beyond the hedonic treadmill: revising the adaptation theory of well-being. , 2006, The American psychologist.

[74]  C. Daniel Batson,et al.  Prosocial Motivation: Is it ever Truly Altruistic? , 1987 .

[75]  A. Stone,et al.  Effects of severe daily events on mood. , 1984, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[76]  P. Lewinsohn,et al.  Unpleasant and pleasant events, and mood. , 1980, Journal of clinical psychology.

[77]  D. Campbell,et al.  Hedonic relativism and planning the good society , 1971 .