Attributing information to different sources: effects on the perceived qualities of information, on the perceived relevance of information, and on attitude formation

What is salient in risk communication, and what is believed, is likely to be related to factors additional to the informational content itself. The social context or derivation of the information are important determinants of receiver reactions, particularly where the potential hazard is characterized as unknown, or where the knowledge base surrounding the potential hazard is perceived to be at the level of society rather than of individual members of the general public (for example, genetic engineering). Experimental data are presented which examine the effect of information source on perceptions of qualities of the information (particularly trust in what is being communicated) and subsequent attitudes towards genetic engineering within the context of food safety. It was found that selfreported trust in hypothetical situations may not equate with behavioural responses to actual information when attributed to a particular source. Source attribution as opposed to anonymity was the most important determinant of perceived informational quality. Public reaction to risk information is likely to be determined by a complex inter-relationship between source and hazard characteristics.

[1]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Causal inferences about communicators and their effect on opinion change , 1978 .

[2]  Frank P. McKenna,et al.  It won't happen to me: Unrealistic optimism or illusion of control? , 1993 .

[3]  W. Gamson,et al.  Media Discourse and Public Opinion on Nuclear Power: A Constructionist Approach , 1989, American Journal of Sociology.

[4]  Sheila Jasanoff,et al.  Bridging the Two Cultures of Risk Analysis1,2 , 1993 .

[5]  N. Weinstein Unrealistic optimism about future life events , 1980 .

[6]  Lynn J. Frewer,et al.  THE INTERRELATIONSHIP BETWEEN PERCEIVED KNOWLEDGE, CONTROL AND RISK ASSOCIATED WITH A RANGE OF FOOD‐RELATED HAZARDS TARGETED AT THE INDIVIDUAL, OTHER PEOPLE AND SOCIETY , 1994 .

[7]  J. Baron,et al.  An analysis of the word-superiority effect☆ , 1973 .

[8]  Robert A. Stallings,et al.  Media Discourse and the Social Construction of Risk , 1990 .

[9]  Ortwin Renn,et al.  Risk communication: Towards a rational discourse with the public , 1992 .

[10]  F. McKenna,et al.  Factors underlying illusory self-assessment of driving skill in males and females. , 1991, Accident Analysis and Prevention.

[11]  Paul Slovic,et al.  Trust as a Determinant of Opposition to a High‐Level Radioactive Waste Repository: Analysis of a Structural Model , 1992 .

[12]  William R. Freudenburg,et al.  Risk and Recreancy: Weber, the Division of Labor, and the Rationality of Risk Perceptions , 1993 .

[13]  P M Sandman,et al.  Optimistic biases in public perceptions of the risk from radon. , 1988, American journal of public health.