Short-latency ocular following in humans is dependent on absolute (rather than relative) binocular disparity

A previous study showed that the initial ocular following responses elicited by sudden motion of a large random-dot pattern were only modestly attenuated when that whole pattern was shifted out of the plane of fixation by altering its horizontal binocular disparity, but the same disparity applied to a restricted region of the dots had a much more powerful effect [Vision Research 41 (2001) 3371]. Thus, if the dots were partitioned into horizontal bands, for example, and alternate bands were moved in opposite directions to the left or right then ocular following was very weak, but if the (conditioning) dots moving in one direction were all shifted out of the plane of fixation (by applying horizontal disparity to them) then strong ocular following was now seen in the direction of motion of the (test) dots in the plane of fixation, i.e., moving images became much less effective when they were given binocular disparity. We sought to determine if the greater impact of disparity with the partitioned images was because there were additional relative disparity cues. We used a similar partitioned display and found that the dependence of ocular following on the absolute disparity of the conditioning stimulus had a Gaussian form with an x-offset that was close to zero disparity and, importantly, this offset was almost unaffected by changing the absolute disparity of the test stimulus. We conclude from this that it is the absolute--rather than the relative--disparity that is important, and that ocular following has a strong preference for moving images whose absolute disparities are close to zero. This is consistent with the idea that ocular following selectively stabilizes the retinal images of objects in and around the plane of fixation and works in harmony with disparity vergence, which uses absolute disparity to bring objects of interest into the plane of fixation [Archives of Ophthalmology 55 (1956) 848].

[1]  F. A. Miles,et al.  Vergence eye movements in response to binocular disparity without depth perception , 1997, Nature.

[2]  G WESTHEIMER,et al.  Eye movement responses to convergence stimuli. , 1956, A.M.A. archives of ophthalmology.

[3]  C Busettini,et al.  Short-latency disparity vergence in humans. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. II. Binocular interactions and sensitivity to binocular disparity. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Response to motion in extrastriate area MSTl: center-surround interactions. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  G. Mackensen Untersuchungen zur Physiologie des optokinetischen Nystagmus , 2004, Albrecht von Graefes Archiv für Ophthalmologie.

[7]  D. Regan,et al.  Necessary conditions for the perception of motion in depth. , 1986, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[8]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Disparity sensitivity of neurons in monkey extrastriate area MST , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[9]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Analysis of local and wide-field movements in the superior temporal visual areas of the macaque monkey , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Organization of Disparity-Selective Neurons in Macaque Area MT , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  R. Andersen,et al.  Center–Surround Antagonism Based on Disparity in Primate Area MT , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  I. P. Howard,et al.  Human optokinetic nystagmus is linked to the stereoscopic system , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  T. Albright,et al.  Image Segmentation Cues in Motion Processing: Implications for Modularity in Vision , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  Aya Takemura,et al.  Visually Driven Eye Movements Elicited at Ultra‐short Latency Are Severely Impaired by MST Lesions , 2002, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[15]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Cortical area MT and the perception of stereoscopic depth , 1998, Nature.

[16]  G. Mackensen Untersuchungen zur Physiologie des optokinetischen Nystagmus , 2004, Albrecht von Graefes Archiv für Ophthalmologie.

[17]  Lance M. Optican,et al.  Unix-based multiple-process system, for real-time data acquisition and control , 1982 .

[18]  J. Allman,et al.  Stimulus specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field: neurophysiological mechanisms for local-global comparisons in visual neurons. , 1985, Annual review of neuroscience.

[19]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[20]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Motion perception during dichoptic viewing of moving random-dot stereograms , 1985, Vision Research.

[21]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The middle temporal visual area in the macaque: Myeloarchitecture, connections, functional properties and topographic organization , 1981, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[22]  D. Robinson,et al.  A METHOD OF MEASURING EYE MOVEMENT USING A SCLERAL SEARCH COIL IN A MAGNETIC FIELD. , 1963, IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering.

[23]  D. Glaser,et al.  Depth discrimination of a line is improved by adding other nearby lines , 1992, Vision Research.

[24]  F. A. Miles,et al.  Single-unit activity in cortical area MST associated with disparity-vergence eye movements: evidence for population coding. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Precise recording of human eye movements , 1975, Vision Research.

[26]  Ian P. Howard,et al.  Human optokinetic nystagmus in response to moving binocularly disparate stimuli , 1987, Vision Research.

[27]  G. Westheimer,et al.  Cooperative neural processes involved in stereoscopic acuity , 1979, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  M. C. Jones,et al.  Spline Smoothing and Nonparametric Regression. , 1989 .

[29]  E. Fitzgibbon,et al.  Short-latency disparity-vergence eye movements in humans: sensitivity to simulated orthogonal tropias , 2003, Vision Research.

[30]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Cortical connections of visual area MT in the macaque , 1986, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[31]  A. Parker,et al.  Binocular Neurons in V1 of Awake Monkeys Are Selective for Absolute, Not Relative, Disparity , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  F A Miles,et al.  Short-latency ocular following responses of monkey. II. Dependence on a prior saccadic eye movement. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The connections of the middle temporal visual area (MT) and their relationship to a cortical hierarchy in the macaque monkey , 1983, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[34]  O. Braddick Segmentation versus integration in visual motion processing , 1993, Trends in Neurosciences.

[35]  R. Born,et al.  Segregation of Object and Background Motion in Visual Area MT Effects of Microstimulation on Eye Movements , 2000, Neuron.

[36]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Response to motion in extrastriate area MSTl: disparity sensitivity. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  S. Yamane,et al.  Neural activity in cortical area MST of alert monkey during ocular following responses. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  A. Parker,et al.  The Precision of Single Neuron Responses in Cortical Area V1 during Stereoscopic Depth Judgments , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  R. Andersen,et al.  Integration of motion and stereopsis in middle temporal cortical area of macaques , 1995, Nature.

[40]  C Busettini,et al.  Short-latency ocular following in humans: sensitivity to binocular disparity , 2001, Vision Research.

[41]  A. Parker,et al.  A specialization for relative disparity in V2 , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  J Allman,et al.  Direction- and Velocity-Specific Responses from beyond the Classical Receptive Field in the Middle Temporal Visual Area (MT) , 1985, Perception.

[43]  F A Miles,et al.  The neural processing of 3‐D visual information: evidence from eye movements , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.

[44]  F. A. Miles,et al.  Short-latency ocular following responses of monkey. I. Dependence on temporospatial properties of visual input. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  F A Miles,et al.  Short latency ocular-following responses in man , 1990, Visual Neuroscience.

[46]  G C DeAngelis,et al.  The physiology of stereopsis. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[47]  G. Mackensen,et al.  Untersuchungen zur Physiologie des optokinetischen Nachnystagmus , 1959, Albrecht von Graefes Archiv für Ophthalmologie.

[48]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Eye movements and stereopsis during dichoptic viewing of moving random-dot stereograms , 1985, Vision Research.

[49]  B. Yandell Spline smoothing and nonparametric regression , 1989 .