The Last Word in Court—A Hidden Disadvantage for the Defense

In the legal systems of most western countries, defense attorneys present their sentencing recommendation after the prosecution has presented its sentencing demands. This procedural sequence for criminal cases is intended to balance the impact of both parties on the judge's final decision. Especially the positioning of the defense's plea at the end of the trial follows the fundamental legal principle “in dubio pro reo.” Research on judgmental anchoring, however, suggests that the standard procedural sequence may in fact work against this principle. Consistent with this implication, the present studies demonstrate that the defense's sentencing recommendation is anchored on, and consequently assimilated toward, the preceding recommendation by the prosecution. This influence prevents the defense attorney from effectively counterbalancing the prosecutor's demand. Instead, the biased defense attorney's recommendation partially mediates the impact of the prosecutor's demand on the judge's decision. These findings suggest that the standard procedural sequence in court may place the defense at a distinct disadvantage.

[1]  John M. Malouff,et al.  Shaping juror attitudes: effects of requesting different damage amounts in personal injury trials , 1989 .

[2]  Glen Whyte,et al.  The Effect of Multiple Anchors on Anchoring in Individual and Group Judgment , 1997 .

[3]  Shelly Chaiken,et al.  Principles of persuasion. , 1996 .

[4]  J. Thibaut,et al.  Procedural Justice: A Psychological Analysis , 1976 .

[5]  F. Strack,et al.  Playing Dice With Criminal Sentences: The Influence of Irrelevant Anchors on Experts’ Judicial Decision Making , 2006, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[6]  Brian H. Bornstein,et al.  The More You Ask For, the More You Get: Anchoring in Personal Injury Verdicts , 1996 .

[7]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  A new look at anchoring effects: basic anchoring and its antecedents. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[8]  Moral Education for a Society in Moral Transition. , 1975 .

[9]  William J. McGuire,et al.  Attitudes and attitude change , 1985 .

[10]  M. Zanna,et al.  Attitudes and Attitude Change , 1993 .

[11]  D. Campbell,et al.  Recency and primacy in persuasion as a function of the timing of speeches and measurements. , 1959, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[12]  V. Hinsz,et al.  Assimilation to Anchors for Damage Awards in a Mock Civil Trial1 , 1995 .

[13]  Mark Snyder,et al.  Behavioral confirmation in social interaction: From social perception to social reality. , 1978 .

[14]  Thomas Mussweiler,et al.  Comparing Is Believing: A Selective Accessibility Model of Judgmental Anchoring , 1999 .

[15]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  Stuart S. Nagel,et al.  Procedural Justice: A Psychological Analysis , 1976 .

[17]  J. Payne,et al.  Juror Judgments in Civil Cases: Effects of Plaintiff's Requests and Plaintiff's Identity on Punitive Damage Awards , 1999 .

[18]  B. Krahé Social Psychological Issues in the Study of Rape , 1991 .

[19]  Thomas Mussweiler,et al.  Subliminal anchoring: Judgmental consequences and underlying mechanisms , 2005 .

[20]  F. Strack,et al.  Explaining the Enigmatic Anchoring Effect: Mechanisms of Selective Accessibility , 1997 .

[21]  F. Strack,et al.  The use of category and exemplar knowledge in the solution of anchoring tasks. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  Eric J. Johnson,et al.  Anchoring, Activation, and the Construction of Values. , 1999, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[23]  Thomas Mussweiler,et al.  Hypothesis-consistent testing and semantic priming in the anchoring paradigm: A selective accessibility model. , 1999 .

[24]  R. H. Willis,et al.  Social Exchange: Advances In Theory And Research , 1981 .

[25]  E. Ebbesen,et al.  The Process of Sentencing Adult Felons , 1981 .

[26]  L. Kohlberg,et al.  The psychology of moral development : the nature and validity of moral stages , 1984 .

[27]  Scott H. Decker,et al.  Law and Society Review , 1979 .

[28]  Duane T. Wegener,et al.  Implications of Attitude Change Theories for Numerical Anchoring: Anchor Plausibility and the Limits of Anchor Effectiveness , 2001 .

[29]  Regina A. Schuller,et al.  Police Responses to Sexual Assault Complaints: The Role of Perpetrator/Complainant Intoxication , 2000, Law and human behavior.

[30]  Norbert Schwarz,et al.  Judgment in a Social Context: Biases, Shortcomings, and the Logic of Conversation , 1994 .

[31]  T. Tyler,et al.  The Social Psychology of Procedural Justice , 1988 .

[32]  Siobhan Chapman Logic and Conversation , 2005 .

[33]  S. Fiske,et al.  The Handbook of Social Psychology , 1935 .

[34]  G. Leventhal What Should Be Done with Equity Theory? New Approaches to the Study of Fairness in Social Relationships. , 1976 .

[35]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[36]  Mandeep K. Dhami,et al.  Psychological Models of Professional Decision Making , 2003, Psychological science.

[37]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty , 1982 .

[38]  Michael A. Becker Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles , 1998 .

[39]  Venkatesh,et al.  Computer Technology Training in the Workplace: A Longitudinal Investigation of the Effect of Mood. , 1999, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[40]  B. Sales The trial process , 1981 .

[41]  S. Diamond Exploring Sources of Sentence Disparity , 1981 .

[42]  J. Robbennolt,et al.  Anchoring in the Courtroom: The Effects of Caps on Punitive Damages , 1999, Law and human behavior.

[43]  N. Epley,et al.  Putting Adjustment Back in the Anchoring and Adjustment Heuristic: Differential Processing of Self-Generated and Experimenter-Provided Anchors , 2001, Psychological science.

[44]  Y. Trope,et al.  Confirmatory and diagnosing strategies in social information gathering. , 1982 .

[45]  John Hogarth,et al.  Sentencing as a Human Process , 1973 .

[46]  Thomas Mussweiler,et al.  Sentencing Under Uncertainty: Anchoring Effects in the Courtroom1 , 2001 .

[47]  E. Higgins,et al.  Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. , 1996 .

[48]  T. Mussweiler Comparison processes in social judgment: mechanisms and consequences. , 2003, Psychological review.

[49]  A. Galinsky,et al.  First offers as anchors: the role of perspective-taking and negotiator focus. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[50]  Michael J. Saks,et al.  Human Information Processing and Adjudication: Trial by Heuristics , 1980 .

[51]  C. Insko,et al.  PRIMACY VERSUS RECENCY IN PERSUASION AS A FUNCTION OF THE TIMING OF ARGUMENTS AND MEASURES. , 1964, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[52]  Roselle L. Wissler,et al.  Be careful what you ask for: the effect of anchors on personal injury damages awards. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[53]  Thomas Mussweiler,et al.  Overcoming the Inevitable Anchoring Effect: Considering the Opposite Compensates for Selective Accessibility , 2000 .

[54]  Daniel Cervone,et al.  Sequence Anchoring and Self-Efficacy: Primacy Effects in the Consideration of Possibilities , 1989 .

[55]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Measures of Anchoring in Estimation Tasks , 1995 .