Attentional Bias to Threat: Roles of Trait Anxiety, Stressful Events, and Awareness

Attentional biases for threat stimuli were assessed in high and low trait anxious subjects (n = 66) using a probe detection task. To examine the effects of trait anxiety and situational stressors, each subject was tested three times: Under no stress, laboratory-induced stress, and examination-induced stress. To evaluate the role of awareness, half the word stimuli were presented very briefly (14 msec) and masked, and the other half were presented for 500 msec without a mask. Results showed that high trait anxious subjects under exam stress showed an attentional bias towards unmasked threat stimuli compared with low trait subjects. This effect was not found under lab-induced stress, suggesting that the attentional bias for unmasked threat in high trait subjects may be a function of a prolonged stressor, rather than a transient increase in state anxiety. The results from the masked exposure condition were not predicted; high trait anxious subjects shifted attention towards the spatial location of threat words despite lack of awareness of their lexical content, but this bias was only apparent in the no-stress condition. The results are discussed in relation to recent cognitive theories of anxiety.

[1]  L. Taylor Relationship between affect and memory: motivation-based selective generation. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  A. Richards,et al.  Colour-identification of differentially valenced words in anxiety , 1989 .

[3]  Recognition and lexical decision without detection: unconscious perception? , 1990 .

[4]  A. Mathews Why worry? The cognitive function of anxiety. , 1990, Behaviour research and therapy.

[5]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[6]  P. V. D. Doef Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders , 1992 .

[7]  K. Mogg,et al.  Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states: a replication. , 1989, Behaviour research and therapy.

[8]  K. Mogg,et al.  Effects of anxiety and awareness on colour-identification latencies for emotional words. , 1993, Behaviour research and therapy.

[9]  Thomas H. Carr,et al.  Task-induced strategies and near-threshold priming: Conscious influences on unconscious perception. , 1989 .

[10]  K. Mogg,et al.  Attentional bias in anxiety and depression: the role of awareness. , 1995, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[11]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Anxiety and the Allocation of Attention to Threat , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[12]  R. McNally,et al.  Selective processing of threat cues in panic disorder. , 1990, Behaviour research and therapy.

[13]  M. Eysenck,et al.  Attentional bias in anxiety: selective search or defective filtering? , 1990, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[14]  G. Bower Mood and memory. , 1981, The American psychologist.

[15]  D. Marlowe,et al.  The Approval Motive: Studies in Evaluative Dependence , 1980 .

[16]  B. Jaffe Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders , 1977 .

[17]  W. Roth,et al.  Selective processing of threat cues in subjects with panic attacks , 1988 .

[18]  A. B. Hill,et al.  Semantic Priming without Awareness: Some Methodological Considerations and Replications , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[19]  K. Mogg,et al.  Processing of emotional information in anxious subjects. , 1990, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[20]  A. Isen,et al.  Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition. , 1984 .

[21]  D. Clark,et al.  Does anxiety lead to selective processing of threat-related information? , 1991, Behaviour research and therapy.

[22]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Attentional bias in emotional disorders. , 1986, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[23]  Donald E. Broadbent,et al.  Anxiety and Attentional Bias: State and Trait , 1988 .

[24]  M. Eysenck,et al.  Effect of psychological treatment on cognitive bias in generalized anxiety disorder. , 1995, Behaviour research and therapy.

[25]  John B. Carroll,et al.  The American Heritage Word Frequency Book , 1971 .

[26]  K. Mogg,et al.  Effects of stress and anxiety on the processing of threat stimuli. , 1990, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.

[27]  K. Mogg,et al.  Subliminal processing of emotional information in anxiety and depression. , 1993, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[28]  T. K. Srull,et al.  Handbook of Social Cognition , 1993 .

[29]  M. Eysenck,et al.  Attentional bias to threat in clinical anxiety states , 1992 .

[30]  A. Richards,et al.  Effects of anxiety and mood manipulation in autobiographical memory. , 1990, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[31]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Micro Experimental Laboratory: An integrated system for IBM PC compatibles , 1988 .

[32]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Discrimination of threat cues without awareness in anxiety states. , 1986, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[33]  E. Reingold,et al.  Recognition and lexical decision without detection: unconscious perception? , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  A. Marcel Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.

[35]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states. , 1985, Behaviour research and therapy.

[36]  C MacLeod,et al.  Anxiety and the selective processing of emotional information: mediating roles of awareness, trait and state variables, and personal relevance of stimulus materials. , 1992, Behaviour research and therapy.

[37]  A. Beck,et al.  An inventory for measuring depression. , 1961, Archives of general psychiatry.

[38]  C. Spielberger,et al.  Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory , 1970 .

[39]  J. Bargh Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. , 1989 .

[40]  J. Williams Cognitive Psychology and Emotional Disorders , 1991 .