The Immersive Virtual Reality Experience: A Typology of Users Revealed Through Multiple Correspondence Analysis Combined with Cluster Analysis Technique

Immersive virtual reality is thought to be advantageous by leading to higher levels of presence. However, and despite users getting actively involved in immersive three-dimensional virtual environments that incorporate sound and motion, there are individual factors, such as age, video game knowledge, and the predisposition to immersion, that may be associated with the quality of virtual reality experience. Moreover, one particular concern for users engaged in immersive virtual reality environments (VREs) is the possibility of side effects, such as cybersickness. The literature suggests that at least 60% of virtual reality users report having felt symptoms of cybersickness, which reduces the quality of the virtual reality experience. The aim of this study was thus to profile the right user to be involved in a VRE through head-mounted display. To examine which user characteristics are associated with the most effective virtual reality experience (lower cybersickness), a multiple correspondence analysis combined with cluster analysis technique was performed. Results revealed three distinct profiles, showing that the PC gamer profile is more associated with higher levels of virtual reality effectiveness, that is, higher predisposition to be immersed and reduced cybersickness symptoms in the VRE than console gamer and nongamer. These findings can be a useful orientation in clinical practice and future research as they help identify which users are more predisposed to benefit from immersive VREs.

[1]  Chwen Jen Chen,et al.  The Theoretical Framework for Designing Desktop Virtual Reality-Based Learning Environments , 2004 .

[2]  Yun Ling,et al.  The relationship between individual characteristics and experienced presence , 2013, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[3]  Marina Krcmar,et al.  Experiencing Presence in Video Games: The Role of Presence Tendencies, Game Experience, Gender, and Time Spent in Play , 2011 .

[4]  Noomi Katz,et al.  Video capture virtual reality as a flexible and effective rehabilitation tool , 2004, Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation.

[5]  Pedro Gamito,et al.  Show me your eyes! The combined use of eye tracking and virtual reality applications for cognitive assessment , 2015, REHAB.

[6]  Bettina A Babbitt,et al.  Questionnaire Construction Manual Annex. Questionnaires: Literature Survey and Bibliography , 1989 .

[7]  S Bangay,et al.  An investigation into factors influencing immersion in interactive virtual reality environments. , 1998, Studies in health technology and informatics.

[8]  R. Clarke,et al.  Theory and Applications of Correspondence Analysis , 1985 .

[9]  Nicole Strangman,et al.  Virtual Reality / Computer Simulations , 2003 .

[10]  Concepción Perpiñá,et al.  Virtual Reality and Psychopathology , 1999, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[11]  Pedro J. Rosa,et al.  Beyond Traditional Clinical Measurements for Screening Fears and Phobias , 2015, IEEE Transactions on Instrumentation and Measurement.

[12]  P. Gamito,et al.  Training presence: the importance of virtual reality experience on the "sense of being there". , 2010, Studies in health technology and informatics.

[13]  Rubén G. Rumbaut,et al.  On the Frontier of Adulthood , 2005 .

[14]  E. Reed The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1989 .

[15]  Brenda K. Wiederhold,et al.  Virtual reality therapy for anxiety disorders: Advances in evaluation and treatment. , 2005 .

[16]  Marko Turpeinen,et al.  Spatial Presence and Emotions during Video Game Playing: Does It Matter with Whom You Play? , 2006, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[17]  E. Pahle,et al.  Virtual reality computer simulation , 2001, Surgical Endoscopy.

[18]  Gregory M. P. O'Hare,et al.  Presence Equation: An Investigation into Cognitive Factors Underlying Presence , 2003, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[19]  Pedro Gamito,et al.  Eliciting Nicotine Craving with Virtual Smoking Cues , 2014, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[20]  Michael J. Singer,et al.  Measuring Presence in Virtual Environments: A Presence Questionnaire , 1998, Presence.

[21]  R. Goebel,et al.  Local Discriminability Determines the Strength of Holistic Processing for Faces in the Fusiform Face Area , 2013, Front. Psychology.

[22]  Jörg Niesenhaus,et al.  Measuring the impact of game controllers on player experience in FPS games , 2011, MindTrek.

[23]  N. Newcombe,et al.  How Important Is the Digital Divide? The Relation of Computer and Videogame Usage to Gender Differences in Mental Rotation Ability , 2005 .

[24]  Mel Slater,et al.  The Influence of Body Movement on Subjective Presence in Virtual Environments , 1998, Hum. Factors.

[25]  Clint A. Bowers,et al.  The Effects of Gender, Age, and Experience on Game Engagement , 2013 .

[26]  Robert S. Kennedy,et al.  Simulator Sickness Questionnaire: An enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness. , 1993 .

[27]  B. G. Witmer,et al.  Immersive Tendency, Feeling of Presence, and Simulator Sickness: Formulation of a Causal Model , 2002 .

[28]  Pedro Gamito,et al.  Attentional orienting to biologically fear-relevant stimuli: data from eye tracking , 2011 .

[29]  Giuseppe Riva,et al.  Being there : the experience of presence in mediated environments , 2003 .

[30]  Tilo Hartmann,et al.  Influence of individual factors on presence , 2008, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[31]  G.G. Robertson,et al.  Three views of virtual reality: nonimmersive virtual reality , 1993, Computer.

[32]  M. Kozhevnikov,et al.  Immersive and non-immersive virtual reality system to learn relative motion concepts , 2013, 2013 3rd Interdisciplinary Engineering Design Education Conference.

[33]  P. Howarth,et al.  Habituation to the Side Effects of Immersion in a Virtual Environment , 2000 .

[34]  Shaun Bangay,et al.  An investigation into factors influencing immersion in interactive virua reality , 1998 .

[35]  Pedro J. Rosa,et al.  Effects of fear-relevant stimuli on attention: Integrating gaze data with subliminal exposure , 2014, 2014 IEEE International Symposium on Medical Measurements and Applications (MeMeA).

[36]  Jeremy N. Bailenson,et al.  A Longitudinal Study of Task Performance, Head Movements, Subjective Report, Simulator Sickness, and Transformed Social Interaction in Collaborative Virtual Environments , 2006, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[37]  Helene S. Wallach,et al.  Personality variables and presence , 2010, Virtual Reality.

[38]  Jonathan Freeman,et al.  A Cross-Media Presence Questionnaire: The ITC-Sense of Presence Inventory , 2001, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[39]  Paul Skalski,et al.  Mapping the road to fun: Natural video game controllers, presence, and game enjoyment , 2011, New Media Soc..

[40]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[41]  Young Youn Kim,et al.  Characteristic changes in the physiological components of cybersickness. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[42]  Helmuth Trefftz,et al.  Design and Implementation of a Low-Cost Projected Virtual Reality System to Support Learning Processes , 2011, HCI.

[43]  Brett D. Jones,et al.  Building novel Web3D user interfaces: a case study from architecture and structural engineering , 2013, Web3D '13.

[44]  Christine Youngblut,et al.  The relationship between presence and performance in virtual environments: results of a VERTS study , 2003, IEEE Virtual Reality, 2003. Proceedings..

[45]  Rupali P. Dhond,et al.  Understanding Immersivity: Image Generation and Transformation Processes in 3D Immersive Environments , 2012, Front. Psychology.