What do fully paralyzed awake humans feel when they attempt to move?

Subjects are able to judge the strength of muscle contraction. In theory, the force of muscular exertion could be perceived either from mechanoreceptor afferents or from knowledge of central motor command (corollary discharge). Sensations of great effort or exerted force have been described by subjects when their limbs were weakened by fatigue or partial paralysis. This has been taken as evidence that effort sensations arise from central motor commands rather than from mechanoreceptor afferent signals produced by muscle contraction. To differentiate between these possibilities, we used neuromuscular block to completely paralyze four waking subjects and required them to attempt maximal contraction of inspiratory muscles and of hand muscles. They were questioned after recovery about what their sensations were when attempting these contractions. None described the sensations of exerted force, great effort, or heaviness, which would have been expected if motor commands alone were the source of these sensations. The contradiction between our findings and those previously reported suggests that the specific neural mechanisms for effort sensations must be reexamined.

[1]  R. Lansing,et al.  'Air hunger' from increased PCO2 persists after complete neuromuscular block in humans. , 1990, Respiration physiology.

[2]  S. Gandevia Roles for perceived voluntary motor commands in motor control , 1987, Trends in Neurosciences.

[3]  S. Gandevia,et al.  Cardiovascular responses and the sense of effort during attempts to contract paralysed muscles: Role of the spinal cord , 1985, Neuroscience Letters.

[4]  S. Gandevia The perception of motor commands or effort during muscular paralysis. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  W Z Rymer,et al.  Abnormal force--EMG relations in paretic limbs of hemiparetic human subjects. , 1981, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[6]  S. Gandevia,et al.  The effect of respiratory muscle fatigue on respiratory sensations. , 1981, Clinical science.

[7]  S. Gandevia,et al.  Changes in the perception of inspiratory resistive loads during partial curarization. , 1980, The Journal of physiology.

[8]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Interpretation of perceived motor commands by reference to afferent signals. , 1978, The Journal of physiology.

[9]  R. C. Emerson,et al.  Paralysis of the awake human: Visual perceptions , 1976, Vision Research.

[10]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Corollary motor discharges and kinaesthesia , 1975, Brain Research.

[11]  P. Matthews,et al.  Mammalian muscle receptors and their central actions , 1974 .

[12]  R. Melzack,et al.  Experimental phantom limbs. , 1973, Experimental neurology.

[13]  T. Clark,et al.  The effect of muscular paralysis induced by tubocurarine on the duration and sensation of breath-holding during hypercapnia. , 1969, Clinical science.

[14]  T. Clark,et al.  The effect of muscular paralysis induced by tubocurarine on the duration and sensation of breath-holding. , 1967, Clinical science.

[15]  C. Smith,et al.  Neuromuscular Pharmacology: Drugs and Muscle Spindles , 1963 .

[16]  L. Goodman,et al.  THE LACK OF CEREBRAL EFFECTS OF d-TOBOCURARINE , 1947, Anesthesiology.

[17]  Ernst Mach,et al.  The Analysis of Sensations. , 1916 .

[18]  P. Roland,et al.  A quantitative analysis of sensations of tension and of kinaesthesia in man. Evidence for a peripherally originating muscular sense and for a sense of effort. , 1977, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[19]  D. McCloskey,et al.  The contribution of muscle afferents to kinaesthesia shown by vibration induced illusions of movement and by the effects of paralysing joint afferents. , 1972, Brain : a journal of neurology.