Underspecification and Aspectual Coercion

In principle, comprehenders might always make immediate commitments to the interpretation of expressions (full commitment) or wait until such decisions are necessary (minimal commitment; Frazier & Rayner, 1990). One interesting case involves decisions about telicity: whether expressions refer to events that are determinate versus indeterminate with respect to an endpoint. Thus, the insect hopped is apparently determinate, but continuing with a clause beginning with until, in which case hopped must be interpreted as an ongoing activity, is possible. Studies using secondary lexical decision and "stop-making-sense" tasks found that comprehenders experienced difficulty with these continuations, compatible with full commitment (Pinango, Zurif, & Jackendoff, 1999; Todorova, Straub, Badecker, & Frank, 2000a, 2000b). However, we report 2 self-paced reading and 2 eye-tracking experiments that indicate readers do not experience any difficulty with these types of mismatches in telicity. We argue that during normal reading, comprehenders do not immediately need to commit fully to the telicity of events and that full commitment may only occur when processing demands induce immediate decisions. We contrast these results with evidence for full commitment in complement coercions, for example, began the book (McElree, Traxler, Pickering, Seely, & Jackendoff, 2001) and other forms of semantic interpretation.

[1]  M M Piñango,et al.  Real-Time Processing Implications of Enriched Composition at the Syntax–Semantics Interface , 1999, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[2]  Lyn Frazier,et al.  Taking on semantic commitments: Processing multiple meanings vs. multiple senses ☆ , 1990 .

[3]  James Pustejovsky,et al.  The Generative Lexicon , 1995, CL.

[4]  Joseph P. Magliano,et al.  Locus of interpretive and inference processes during text comprehension: A comparison of gaze durations and word reading times. , 1993 .

[5]  Liina Pylkkänen,et al.  Distinct effects of semantic plausibility and semantic composition in MEG , 2004 .

[6]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  The architecture of language , 2000 .

[7]  Martin J. Pickering,et al.  A time course analysis of enriched composition , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[8]  M. Pickering,et al.  Processing ambiguous verbs: evidence from eye movements. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[9]  Ray Jackendoff,et al.  The Architecture of the Language Faculty , 1996 .

[10]  Brian McElree,et al.  Reading time evidence for enriched composition , 2001, Cognition.

[11]  K. Rayner Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.

[12]  J. Fodor,et al.  The Psychology of Language , 1974 .

[13]  G. Seth Psychology of Language , 1968, Nature.

[14]  Robert Frank,et al.  Aspectual Coercion and the Online Computation of Sentential Aspect , 2000 .

[15]  M. Mattson,et al.  From words to meaning: A semantic illusion , 1981 .

[16]  Lyn Frazier,et al.  Taking on semantic commitments, II: collective versus distributive readings , 1999, Cognition.

[17]  R. Burchfield Frequency Analysis of English Usage: Lexicon and Grammar. By W. Nelson Francis and Henry Kučera with the assistance of Andrew W. Mackie. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. 1982. x + 561 , 1985 .

[18]  Arthur C. Graesser,et al.  Component processes in text comprehension and some of their interactions , 1985 .

[19]  M J Pickering,et al.  The processing of metonymy: evidence from eye movements. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[20]  M A Just,et al.  A theory of reading: from eye fixations to comprehension. , 1980, Psychological review.

[21]  Martin J. Pickering,et al.  Coercion in sentence processing: evidence from eye-movements and self-paced reading , 2002 .

[22]  WILLIAM MARSLEN-WILSON,et al.  Linguistic Structure and Speech Shadowing at Very Short Latencies , 1973, Nature.

[23]  Lance J. Rips,et al.  The Time-Course and Cost of Telicity Inferences , 2004 .

[24]  Alexander Pollatsek,et al.  The processing of derived and inflected suffixed words during reading , 2000 .

[25]  Liina Pylkkänen,et al.  Chapter 14 – The Syntax-Semantics Interface: On-Line Composition of Sentence Meaning , 2006 .

[26]  P. C. Wason,et al.  The Processing of Positive and Negative Information , 1959 .

[27]  Martin J. Pickering,et al.  Deferred Interpretations: Why Starting Dickens is Taxing but Reading Dickens Isn't , 2006, Cogn. Sci..

[28]  Stephen B. Barton,et al.  A case study of anomaly detection: Shallow semantic processing and cohesion establishment , 1993, Memory & cognition.

[29]  Martin J. Pickering,et al.  Context effects in coercion: Evidence from eye movements , 2005 .

[30]  Martin J. Pickering,et al.  The difficulty of coercion: A response to de Almeida , 2005, Brain and Language.

[31]  Michael K. Tanenhaus,et al.  Verb Argument Structure in Parsing and Interpretation: Evidence from wh-Questions , 1995 .