What's Key for Key? The Krumhansl-Schmuckler Key-Finding Algorithm Reconsidered

This study examines the Krumhansl-Schmuckler key-finding model, in which the distribution of pitch classes in a piece is compared with an ideal distribution or "key profile" for each key. Several changes are proposed. First, the formula used for the matching process is somewhat simplified. Second, alternative values are proposed for the key profiles themselves. Third, rather than summing the durations of all events of each pitch class, the revised model divides the piece into short segments and labels each pitch class as present or absent in each segment. Fourth, a mechanism for modulation is proposed; a penalty is imposed for changing key from one segment to the next. An implementation of this model was subjected to two tests. First, the model was tested on the fugue subjects from Bach's Well-Tempered Clavier; the model's performance on this corpus is compared with the performances of other models. Second, the model was tested on a corpus of excerpts from the Kostka and Payne harmony textbook (as analyzed by Kostka). Several problems with the modified algorithm are discussed, concerning the rate of modulation, the role of harmony in key finding, and the role of pitch "spellings." The model is also compared with Huron and Parncutt's exponential decay model. The tests presented here suggest that the key-profile model, with the modifications proposed, can provide a highly successful approach to key finding.

[1]  William Forde Thompson,et al.  Perceived Key Movement in Four- Voice Harmony and Single Voices , 1992 .

[2]  Piet G. Vos,et al.  A parallel-processing key-finding model , 1996 .

[3]  David Temperley,et al.  An Algorithm for Harmonic Analysis , 1997 .

[4]  D J Mewhort,et al.  Perception of structure in short melodic sequences. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  J. Bharucha Music Cognition and Perceptual Facilitation: A Connectionist Framework , 1987 .

[6]  R. G. Crowder,et al.  Perception of the Major/Minor Distinction: IV. Emotional Connotations in Young Children , 1990 .

[7]  L. Cuddy,et al.  Melody recognition: the experimental application of musical rules. , 1979, Canadian journal of psychology.

[8]  Fred Lerdahl,et al.  Tonal Pitch Space , 2001 .

[9]  Richard Parncutt,et al.  AN IMPROVED MODEL OF TONALITY PERCEPTION INCORPORATING PITCH SALIENCE AND ECHOIC MEMORY , 1993 .

[10]  Charles Rosen,et al.  The Classical Style: Haydn, Mozart, Beethoven , 1971 .

[11]  Mark Steedman,et al.  On Interpreting Bach , 1987 .

[12]  D. Butler,et al.  Describing the Perception of Tonality in Music: A Critique of the Tonal Hierarchy Theory and a Proposal for a Theory of Intervallic Rivalry , 1989 .

[13]  Hermann Keller,et al.  The well-tempered clavier by Johann Sebastian Bach , 1976 .

[14]  C. Krumhansl,et al.  Tracing the dynamic changes in perceived tonal organization in a spatial representation of musical keys. , 1982 .

[15]  Helen Brown,et al.  The Interplay of Set Content and Temporal Context in a Functional Theory of Tonality Perception , 1988 .

[16]  C. Krumhansl Cognitive Foundations of Musical Pitch , 1990 .

[17]  M. Jones,et al.  Musical and Temporal Influences on Key Discovery , 1994 .

[18]  Daniel Dominic Sleator,et al.  Modeling Meter and Harmony: A Preference-Rule Approach , 1999, Computer Music Journal.

[19]  Nicholas Cook The Perception of Large-Scale Tonal Closure , 1987 .

[20]  Terry Winograd,et al.  Linguistics and the computer analysis of tonal harmony , 1968 .

[21]  R. Jackendoff,et al.  A Generative Theory of Tonal Music , 1985 .

[22]  D. C. Howell Statistical Methods for Psychology , 1987 .

[23]  M. P. Kastner Perception of major/minor : IV. Emotional connotations in young children , 1990 .

[24]  S. R. Holtzman A program for key determination , 1977 .

[25]  Desmond Shawe-Taylor,et al.  The Classical Style , 1977 .