Comparing Three Cognitive Biases for Alcohol Cues in Alcohol Dependence

Aims There is accumulating evidence that automatic processes play a large role in alcohol dependence, which may be related to alcohol craving and consumption. The aim of this study is to investigate associations between cognitive biases in alcohol-dependent patients, and how these measures relate to drinking behavior. Methods Thirty alcohol-dependent patients and 15 healthy controls (matched for age, intelligence and education; all male) completed three cognitive bias tasks: the Implicit Association Test (IAT: alcohol-approach association), Approach Avoidance Task (AAT: alcohol approach bias) and Dot Probe Task (DPT: alcohol attentional bias). Task scores were compared between groups and correlated with each other, as well as with craving scores and drinking behavior. Results Patients with alcohol dependence showed stronger alcohol-approach associations on the IAT compared with controls, but there were no group differences for approach or attentional biases. Within the patient group, the alcohol approach bias (AAT) correlated positively with the attend-alcohol attentional bias (DPT), but negatively with alcohol-approach associations (IAT). IAT scores were positively associated with lifetime alcohol intake. Conclusions This study demonstrates for the first time that alcohol-dependent patients have stronger alcohol-approach association scores on the IAT as compared to controls, and that this bias is associated with drinking behavior. Despite the absence of group differences for the approach and attentional biases, the positive correlation between these biases in alcoholics is in line with incentive salience models of addiction that propose that attentional and approach tendencies have a common underlying mechanism, distinct from that underlying alcohol-approach associations measured by the IAT. Short Summary The study investigates associations between cognitive biases involving alcohol cues. Patients with alcohol dependence showed stronger alcohol-approach associations on an Implicit Association Test than controls, but there were no group differences for approach or attentional biases. Alcohol-approach and attentional bias correlated positively in the patient group.

[1]  R. Wiers,et al.  Implicit and explicit alcohol-related cognitions in heavy and light drinkers , 2002 .

[2]  Henrik Walter,et al.  Effects of cognitive bias modification training on neural alcohol cue reactivity in alcohol dependence. , 2015, The American journal of psychiatry.

[3]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[4]  Ann-Christine Ehlis,et al.  Prefrontal correlates of approach preferences for alcohol stimuli in alcohol dependence , 2014, Addiction biology.

[5]  H A Skinner,et al.  Reliability of alcohol use indices. The Lifetime Drinking History and the MAST. , 1982, Journal of studies on alcohol.

[6]  Reinout W Wiers,et al.  Reaching out towards cannabis: approach-bias in heavy cannabis users predicts changes in cannabis use , 2011, Addiction.

[7]  Reinout W. Wiers,et al.  Approach bias modification in alcohol dependence: Do clinical effects replicate and for whom does it work best? , 2013, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[8]  T. Duka,et al.  Avoidance of alcohol-related stimuli in alcohol-dependent inpatients. , 2007, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[9]  K. Mogg,et al.  Do the affective properties of smoking-related cues influence attentional and approach biases in cigarette smokers? , 2008, Journal of psychopharmacology.

[10]  Matt Field,et al.  Attentional biases in abstinent alcoholics and their association with craving. , 2013, Psychology of addictive behaviors : journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.

[11]  M. Field,et al.  To Approach or Avoid Alcohol? Automatic and Self-Reported Motivational Tendencies in Alcohol Dependence , 2012, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[12]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Consequential validity of the implicit association test: comment on Blanton and Jaccard (2006). , 2006, The American psychologist.

[13]  K. Ochsner,et al.  Implicit Alcohol Approach and Avoidance Tendencies Predict Future Drinking in Problem Drinkers. , 2016, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[14]  S. Shiffman,et al.  Attentional bias predicts outcome in smoking cessation. , 2003, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[15]  D. Sheehan,et al.  The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. , 1998, The Journal of clinical psychiatry.

[16]  Reinout W Wiers,et al.  Implicit cognition and addiction: a tool for explaining paradoxical behavior. , 2010, Annual review of clinical psychology.

[17]  Soyoung Q. Park,et al.  Neural Correlates of Alcohol-Approach Bias in Alcohol Addiction: the Spirit is Willing but the Flesh is Weak for Spirits , 2014, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[18]  T. Gladwin Attentional bias variability and cued attentional bias for alcohol stimuli , 2017 .

[19]  M. Field,et al.  The validity of different measures of automatic alcohol action tendencies. , 2015, Psychology of addictive behaviors : journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.

[20]  Reinout W Wiers,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience of cognitive retraining for addiction medicine: From mediating mechanisms to questions of efficacy. , 2016, Progress in brain research.

[21]  Soyoung Q. Park,et al.  Effects of cognitive bias modification training on neural signatures of alcohol approach tendencies in male alcohol‐dependent patients , 2015, Addiction biology.

[22]  R. Wiers,et al.  Retraining automatic action-tendencies to approach alcohol in hazardous drinkers. , 2010, Addiction.

[23]  Mike Rinck,et al.  Mediation of cognitive bias modification for alcohol addiction via stimulus-specific alcohol avoidance association. , 2015, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[24]  R. Wiers,et al.  Alcohol-related biases in selective attention and action tendency make distinct contributions to dysregulated drinking behaviour. , 2013, Addiction.

[25]  R. Wiers,et al.  Relatively strong automatic appetitive action‐tendencies in male carriers of the OPRM1 G‐allele , 2009, Genes, brain, and behavior.

[26]  Rajita Sinha,et al.  Sex differences in neural responses to stress and alcohol context cues , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[27]  Reinout W. Wiers,et al.  Should We Train Alcohol-Dependent Patients to Avoid Alcohol? , 2013, Front. Psychiatry.

[28]  Karl Mann,et al.  Avoidance of alcohol-related stimuli increases during the early stage of abstinence in alcohol-dependent patients. , 2009, Alcohol and alcoholism.

[29]  P. Willner,et al.  A comparison of two alcohol craving questionnaires. , 1998, Addiction.

[30]  K. Mogg,et al.  Attentional and approach biases for smoking cues in smokers: an investigation of competing theoretical views of addiction , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[31]  Xavier Noël,et al.  Time course of attention for alcohol cues in abstinent alcoholic patients: the role of initial orienting. , 2006, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[32]  T. Palfai,et al.  Compelled to consume: the Implicit Association Test and automatic alcohol motivation. , 2006, Psychology of addictive behaviors : journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.

[33]  R. Wiers,et al.  Retraining Automatic Action Tendencies Changes Alcoholic Patients’ Approach Bias for Alcohol and Improves Treatment Outcome , 2011, Psychological science.

[34]  O. Aasland,et al.  Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO Collaborative Project on Early Detection of Persons with Harmful Alcohol Consumption--II. , 1993, Addiction.

[35]  T. Palfai,et al.  Alcohol-related motivational tendencies in hazardous drinkers: assessing implicit response tendencies using the modified-IAT. , 2003, Behaviour research and therapy.

[36]  A. Kaufman,et al.  Assessing Adolescent and Adult Intelligence , 1990 .

[37]  R. Wiers,et al.  ranscranial direct current stimulation , implicit alcohol associations nd craving ess , 2015 .

[38]  Reinout W. Wiers,et al.  Automatic approach bias towards smoking cues is present in smokers but not in ex-smokers , 2013, Psychopharmacology.

[39]  L. Hogarth,et al.  The role of attentional bias in obesity and addiction. , 2016, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[40]  Gladys N. Pachas,et al.  Brain Reactivity to Smoking Cues Prior to Smoking Cessation Predicts Ability to Maintain Tobacco Abstinence , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.

[41]  N. Volkow,et al.  Neurobiology of addiction: a neurocircuitry analysis. , 2016, The lancet. Psychiatry.

[42]  C. Feyerabend,et al.  Determinants and effects of attentional bias in smokers. , 2000, Psychology of addictive behaviors : journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.

[43]  B. Hommel,et al.  Motivational Mechanisms Underlying the Approach Bias to Cigarettes , 2013 .

[44]  Matt Field,et al.  Eye movements to smoking-related pictures in smokers: relationship between attentional biases and implicit and explicit measures of stimulus valence. , 2003, Addiction.

[45]  R. Wiers,et al.  The potential role of temporal dynamics in approach biases: delay-dependence of a general approach bias in an alcohol approach-avoidance task , 2014, Front. Psychol..