Report Consensus Decision Making by Fish

Australia School of Biological SciencesUniversity of SydneySydney, NSW 2006AustraliaSummaryDecisions reached through consensus are often more accu-rate, because they efficiently utilize the diverse informationpossessed by group members [1–3]. A trust in consensusdecision making underlies many of our democratic politicaland judicial institutions [4], as well as the design of webtools such as Google, Wikipedia, and prediction markets[5, 6]. In theory,consensus for the option favored by the ma-jorityofgroupmemberswillleadto improveddecision-mak-ing accuracy as group size increases [2, 4]. Although group-living animals are known to utilize social information [7–10],little is known about whether or not decision accuracy in-creases with group size. In order to reach consensus, groupmembersmustbeabletointegratethedisparateinformationthey possess. Positive feedback, resulting from copyingothers, can spread information quickly through the group,but it can also result in all individuals making the same,possiblyincorrect,choice[8,11,12].Ontheotherhand,ifin-dividuals never copy each other, their decision making re-mains independent and they fail to benefit from informationexchange [4]. Here, we show how small groups of stickle-backs (Gasterosteus aculeatus) reach consensus whenchoosing which of two replica fish to follow. As group sizeincreases,thefishmakemoreaccuratedecisions,becomingbetter at discriminating subtle phenotypic differences of thereplicas. A simple quorum rule proves sufficient to explainour observations, suggesting that animals can make accu-ratedecisions without theneedforcomplicatedcomparisonoftheinformationtheypossess.Furthermore,althoughsub-mission to peers can lead to occasional cascades of incor-rect decisions, these can be explained as a byproduct ofwhat is usually accurate consensus decision making.Results and DiscussionTo investigate consensus decision making, we presentedgroups of three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus;a common freshwater fish) with images of conspecifics withdiffering phenotypic traits. An individual’s appearance canconvey information to, and thus have important fitness conse-quences for, an observer [7]. For example, abdomen profilecan imply foraging success, whereas color may relate tofishes’ health and small black spots could indicate infestationby a parasite. In our experiment, we chose four phenotypesthatwerelikelytoconveyinformation:size,corpulence,shade,and spottiness. Eleven different images were created, depict-ingsmall,medium,orlarge;fat,medium,orthin;light,medium,ordark;andspottedorplain(seeExperimentalProcedures).Ineach trial, we presented the focal fish with a choice betweentwo replicas with different traits.Figure1givesthedistributionofthenumberoffishfollowingthemore attractive replica fish for experimental trials with one,two, four, and eight fish and the large versus medium and darkversus light treatments. For single individuals, one of thereplicas was always more attractive (in the sense that it wasfollowed more often) than the other. This bias was preservedas group size increased, with the majority of fish followingthe image that was more attractive in the single fish trials. Asgroup size increases, the distribution of the number of fish fol-lowing the popular leader takes a J shape (test of fit of a bino-mial distribution is significant to p < 0.001 for all treatments forgroup sizes of four and eight). In the majority of trials, either allor all but one of the fish followed the more attractive leader,whereas in a substantial minority of trials, all or all but one ofthe fish followed the least attractive leader. Similar resultswere seen across all experimental treatments (see Figure S2,available online): fish in the same trial tend to follow thesame leader, and the proportion of fish following the moreattractive leader increases with group size. These J-shapeddistributions are characteristic of positive feedback andcopying [11, 13, 14].In order to make more concrete statements about the typeof decision making occurring in these groups, we give threealternative hypotheses about the probability that a fish ina group of size n with replica treatment jfollow the more willattractive replica in that treatment, p

[1]  S. Milgram,et al.  GROUP PRESSURE AND ACTION AGAINST A PERSON. , 1964, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[2]  Sasha R. X. Dall,et al.  Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. , 2005, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[3]  José Halloy,et al.  Collegial decision making based on social amplification leads to optimal group formation. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  T. Valone,et al.  Public Information: From Nosy Neighbors to Cultural Evolution , 2004, Science.

[5]  Matthew J. Salganik,et al.  Experimental Study of Inequality and Unpredictability in an Artificial Cultural Market , 2006, Science.

[6]  D. Sumpter The principles of collective animal behaviour , 2006, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  K. Laland,et al.  Social transmission of maladaptive information in the guppy , 1998 .

[8]  Christian List,et al.  Democracy in animal groups: a political science perspective. , 2004, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[9]  Eamonn B. Mallon,et al.  An agent-based model of collective nest choice by the ant Temnothorax albipennis , 2005, Animal Behaviour.

[10]  I. Couzin,et al.  Effective leadership and decision-making in animal groups on the move , 2005, Nature.

[11]  S. Pratt,et al.  A tunable algorithm for collective decision-making , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  Eamonn B. Mallon,et al.  Quorum sensing, recruitment, and collective decision-making during colony emigration by the ant Leptothorax albipennis , 2002, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[13]  Andrew M Simons,et al.  Many wrongs: the advantage of group navigation. , 2004, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[14]  S. Pratt,et al.  Quorum responses and consensus decision making , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[15]  L. Conradt,et al.  Consensus decision making in animals. , 2005, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[16]  S. Milgram,et al.  Note on the drawing power of crowds of different size. , 1969 .

[17]  Solomon E. Asch,et al.  Opinions and Social Pressure , 1955 .

[18]  Paul J. B. Hart,et al.  Quorum decision-making facilitates information transfer in fish shoals , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[19]  Guy Cowlishaw,et al.  When to use social information: the advantage of large group size in individual decision making , 2007, Biology Letters.

[20]  S. Milgram BEHAVIORAL STUDY OF OBEDIENCE. , 1963, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[21]  J. Deneubourg,et al.  Self-organized shortcuts in the Argentine ant , 1989, Naturwissenschaften.

[22]  Kevin N Laland,et al.  Nine-spined sticklebacks exploit the most reliable source when public and private information conflict , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[23]  Paul J. B. Hart,et al.  Foraging nine-spined sticklebacks prefer to rely on public information over simpler social cues , 2005 .

[24]  T. Seeley,et al.  Group decision making in nest-site selection by honey bees , 2004 .