Base-Rate Information in Consumer Attributions of Product-Harm Crises

Consumers spontaneously construct attributions for negative events such as product-harm crises. Base-rate information influences these attributions. The research findings suggest that for brands with positive prior beliefs, a high (vs. low) base rate of product-harm crises leads to less blame if the crisis is said to be similar to others in the industry (referred to as the “discounting effect”). However, in the absence of similarity information, a low (vs. high) base rate of crises leads to less blame toward the brand (referred to as the “subtyping effect”). For brands with negative prior beliefs, the extent of blame attributed to the brand is unaffected by the base-rate and similarity information. Importantly, the same base-rate information may have a different effect on the attribution of a subsequent crisis depending on whether discounting or subtyping occurred in the attribution of the first crisis. Consumers who discount a first crisis also tend to discount a second crisis for the same brand, whereas consumers who subtype a first crisis are unlikely to subtype again.

[1]  Harald J. van Heerde,et al.  Rising from the Ashes: How Brands and Categories Can Overcome Product-Harm Crises , 2013 .

[2]  Sabine A. Einwiller,et al.  How to Save Your Brand In the Face of Crisis , 2010 .

[3]  Larry Percy,et al.  Strategic Brand Management , 2007 .

[4]  E. Cowley Views from consumers next in line: The fundamental attribution error in a service setting , 2005 .

[5]  F. Fincham,et al.  Attribution processes in distressed and nondistressed couples: 3. causal and responsibility attributions for spouse behavior , 1987, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[6]  N. Dawar,et al.  Corporate social responsibility and consumers' attributions and brand evaluations in a product–harm crisis , 2004 .

[7]  D. Hilton Knowledge-Based Causal Attribution : The Abnormal Conditions Focus Model , 2004 .

[8]  Marija J. Norusis,et al.  SPSS 16.0 Statistical Procedures Companion , 2003 .

[9]  Bertram Gawronski Implicational schemata and the correspondence bias: on the diagnostic value of situationally constrained behavior. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  O. Ybarra Naive causal understanding of valenced behaviors and its implications for social information processing. , 2002, Psychological bulletin.

[11]  D. Gilbert Heuristics and Biases: Inferential Correction , 2002 .

[12]  Miles Hewstone,et al.  Subtyping and Subgrouping: Processes for the Prevention and Promotion of Stereotype Change , 2001 .

[13]  M. Pillutla,et al.  Impact of Product-Harm Crises on Brand Equity: The Moderating Role of Consumer Expectations , 2000 .

[14]  Barton A. Weitz,et al.  The Effects of Brandbconsumer Relationships Upon Consumers Attributions and Reactions , 1999 .

[15]  T. Berard Attributions and Avowals of Motive in the Study of Deviance: Resource or Topic? , 1998 .

[16]  H. S. Krishnan Characteristics of memory associations: A consumer-based brand equity perspective , 1996 .

[17]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  A disconfirmation bias in the evaluation of arguments. , 1996 .

[18]  Andrew F. Simon,et al.  Enhancing the Impact of Counterstereotypic Information: Dispositional Attributions for Deviance , 1996 .

[19]  G. Johar Intended and Unintended Effects of Corrective Advertising on Beliefs and Evaluations: An Exploratory Analysis , 1996 .

[20]  Jonathan J. Koehler,et al.  The Base Rate Fallacy Reconsidered: Normative, Descriptive and Methodological Challenges , 1996 .

[21]  D. Hilton,et al.  Processes of causal explanation and dispositional attribution. , 1995 .

[22]  D. Gilbert,et al.  The correspondence bias. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[23]  G. Reeder Trait-Behavior Relations and Dispositional Inference , 1993 .

[24]  C. H. Hansen,et al.  The Effects of Stereotype Consistency and Consensus Information on Predictions of Performance , 1993 .

[25]  P. Herr,et al.  Effects of Word-of-Mouth and Product-Attribute Information on Persuasion: An Accessibility-Diagnosticity Perspective , 1991 .

[26]  A. L. McGill Predicting consumers' reactions to product failure: Do responsibility judgments follow from consumers' causal explanations? , 1991 .

[27]  Jean B. Romeo The Effect of Negative Information on the Evaluations of Brand Extensions and the Family Brand , 1991 .

[28]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Social cognition, 2nd ed. , 1991 .

[29]  Z. Kunda,et al.  The case for motivated reasoning. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.

[30]  Maya Bar-Hillel,et al.  Back to base rates. , 1990 .

[31]  F D Fincham,et al.  Attributions in marriage: review and critique. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.

[32]  J. Bettman,et al.  The Effects of Brand Positioning Strategies on Consumers’ Brand and Category Perceptions: Some Insights from Schema Research , 1989 .

[33]  Chezy Ofir,et al.  Effects of cue consistency and value on base-rate utilization. , 1989 .

[34]  Daniel T. Gilbert,et al.  Thinking lightly about others: Automatic components of the social inference process. , 1989 .

[35]  D. Gilbert,et al.  On cognitive busyness: When person perceivers meet persons perceived. , 1988 .

[36]  V. Folkes Recent Attribution Research in Consumer Behavior: A Review and New Directions , 1988 .

[37]  F. Kardes Base Rate Information, Causal Inference, and Preference , 1988 .

[38]  E. E. Jones Attribution: Perceiving the Causes of Behavior , 1987 .

[39]  Y. Trope Identification and Inferential Processes in Dispositional Attribution. , 1986 .

[40]  V. Folkes,et al.  Buyers' and Sellers' Explanations for Product Failure: Who Done It? , 1986 .

[41]  Chezy Ofir,et al.  Context Effects on Judgment under Uncertainty , 1984 .

[42]  V. Folkes Consumer Reactions to Product Failure: An Attributional Approach , 1984 .

[43]  J. V. Wood,et al.  Attributions, beliefs about control, and adjustment to breast cancer. , 1984, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[44]  Cognitive Structure and Stereotype Change , 1983 .

[45]  E. Higgins,et al.  Consensus information and fundamental attribution error: The role of development and in-group versus out-group knowledge. , 1982 .

[46]  Paul T. P. Wong,et al.  When people ask "why" questions, and the heuristics of attributional search. , 1981 .

[47]  William B. Locander,et al.  Attribution Theory and Advertising Effectiveness , 1980 .

[48]  M. Bar-Hillel The base-rate fallacy in probability judgments. , 1980 .

[49]  H. Kelley,et al.  Attribution theory and research. , 1980, Annual review of psychology.

[50]  Saul M. Kassin,et al.  Consensus information, prediction, and causal attribution: A review of the literature and issues. , 1979 .

[51]  P. Pilkonis The Influence of Consensus Information and Prior Expectations on Dispositional and Situational Attributions , 1977 .

[52]  D. Ruble,et al.  Use of consensus information in causal attributions as a function of temporal presentation and availability of direct information. , 1976 .

[53]  Eugene Borgida,et al.  Attribution and the psychology of prediction. , 1975 .

[54]  H. Kelley Attribution theory in social psychology , 1967 .

[55]  E. S. Conklin Human Motivation , 1932, The Journal of Religion.