Contrasting five different theories of letter position coding: evidence from orthographic similarity effects.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Manuel Perea,et al. Transposed-Letter Confusability Effects in Masked Form Priming , 2003 .
[2] S. Andrews. The effect of orthographic similarity on lexical retrieval: Resolving neighborhood conflicts , 1997 .
[3] K. Paap,et al. Neighborhood frequency does affect performance in the Reicher task: encoding or decision? , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] Ken N. Seergobin,et al. On the association between connectionism and data: Are a few words necessary? , 1990 .
[5] C. Whitney. How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective literature review , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[6] K I Forster,et al. The prime lexicality effect: form-priming as a function of prime awareness, lexical status, and discrimination difficulty. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[7] M. Zorzi,et al. Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual-process model. , 1998 .
[8] M Taft,et al. Bodies, antibodies, and neighborhood-density effects in masked form priming. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[9] Jonathan Grainger,et al. The Time Course of Orthographic and Phonological Code Activation , 2006 .
[10] W. Estes,et al. Serial position functions for letter identification at brief and extended exposure durations , 1976 .
[11] M Brysbaert,et al. Prelexical phonological coding of visual words in Dutch: Automatic after all , 2001, Memory & cognition.
[12] J. Requin. Attention and Performance VII , 1980 .
[13] James L. McClelland,et al. Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .
[14] K. Rayner. Eye Guidance in Reading: Fixation Locations within Words , 1979, Perception.
[15] James L. McClelland,et al. Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.
[16] Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al. Lesioning an attractor network: investigations of acquired dyslexia , 1991 .
[17] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: insights from connectionist models. , 1999, Psychological review.
[18] J Grainger,et al. Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: a multiple read-out model. , 1996, Psychological review.
[19] A. Treisman,et al. Illusory words: the roles of attention and of top-down constraints in conjoining letters to form words. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[20] M. Mozer,et al. Perceptual interactions in two-word displays: familiarity and similarity effects. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[21] T. M. Ellison,et al. Eye-fixation behavior, lexical storage, and visual word recognition in a split processing model. , 2000, Psychological review.
[22] M Perea,et al. Repetition and form priming interact with neighborhood density at a brief stimulus onset asynchrony , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[23] A. Jacobs,et al. Localist connectionist approaches to human cognition , 1998 .
[24] Kenneth I Forster,et al. DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy , 2003, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[25] J. Grainger,et al. Letter position coding in printed word perception: Effects of repeated and transposed letters , 2004 .
[26] S. M. Chambers. Letter and order information in lexical access , 1979 .
[27] S. Andrews. Lexical Retrieval and Selection Processes: Effects of Transposed-Letter Confusability , 1996 .
[28] Michael C. Mozer,et al. Perception of multiple objects - a connectionist approach , 1991, Neural network modeling and connectionism.
[29] Padraic Monaghan,et al. The Computational Exploration of Visual Word Recognition in a Split Model , 2001, Neural Computation.
[30] James L. McClelland,et al. A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.
[31] G. Humphreys,et al. The Use of Abstract Graphemic Information in Lexical Access , 1981 .
[32] Sally Andrews,et al. Interactive Activation Accounts of Morphological Decomposition: Finding the Trap in Mousetrap? , 1999, Brain and Language.
[33] Sachiko Kinoshita,et al. Masked priming : the state of the art , 2003 .
[34] Jonathan Grainger,et al. Modeling letter position coding in printed word perception , 2004 .
[35] Randolph G. Bias,et al. Word recognition inside out and outside in , 1981 .
[36] J. Grainger,et al. Does the huamn mnid raed wrods as a wlohe? , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[37] A. Jacobs,et al. On the role of competing word units in visual word recognition: The neighborhood frequency effect , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.
[38] Philip T Quinlan,et al. Orthographic processing in visual word identification , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.
[39] Jonathan Grainger,et al. Letter position information and printed word perception: the relative-position priming constraint. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[40] K I Forster,et al. The potential for experimenter bias effects in word recognition experiments , 2000, Memory & cognition.
[41] Michael E. J. Masson,et al. A Retrospective View of Masked Priming : Toward a Unified Account of Masked and Long-Term Repetition Priming , 2003 .
[42] C. Davis,et al. More words in the neighborhood: Interference in lexical decision due to deletion neighbors , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[43] J. Grainger,et al. Priming word recognition with orthographic neighbors: effects of relative prime-target frequency. , 1990 .
[44] W A Wickelgren,et al. Auditory or articulatory coding in verbal short-term memory. , 1969, Psychological review.
[45] K. Forster,et al. Masked priming with graphemically related forms: Repetition or partial activation? , 1987 .
[46] J Grainger,et al. The role of letter identity and letter position in orthographic priming , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[47] M. Taft,et al. Lack of Phonological Mediation in a Semantic Categorization Task , 1998 .
[48] C. Perfetti,et al. Phonemic activation during the first 40 ms of word identification: Evidence from backward masking and priming , 1991 .
[49] Manuel Perea,et al. Orthographic Neighbours are not all Equal: Evidence using an Identification Technique , 1998, Language and Cognitive Processes.
[50] M. Brysbaert,et al. Neighborhood-frequency effects when primes and targets are of different lengths , 2000, Psychological research.
[51] Michael Garman,et al. Psycholinguistics: Accessing the mental lexicon , 1990 .
[52] C. Davis. N-Watch: A program for deriving neighborhood size and other psycholinguistic statistics , 2005, Behavior research methods.
[53] A. Jacobs,et al. Neighborhood frequency effects and letter visibility in visual word recognition , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[54] Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al. Identification, localization, and “iconic memory”: An evaluation of the bar-probe task , 1981, Memory & cognition.
[55] James L. McClelland,et al. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .
[56] Jonathan Grainger,et al. Phonology and orthography in visual word recognition , 1991 .
[57] K. Forster. Form-priming with masked primes: The best match hypothesis. , 1987 .
[58] B. Bergum,et al. Attention and Performance VI , 1978 .
[59] R S Berndt,et al. A new model of letter string encoding: simulating right neglect dyslexia. , 1999, Progress in brain research.
[60] P. Zwitserlood,et al. Morphological and orthographic similarity in visual word recognition. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[61] James L. McClelland,et al. On learning the past-tenses of English verbs: implicit rules or parallel distributed processing , 1986 .
[62] J. Bowers,et al. Automatic semantic activation of embedded words: Is there a hat in that? , 2005 .
[63] MICHAEL P. A. PAGE. Modelling the Perception of Musical Sequences with Self-organizing Neural Networks , 1994, Connect. Sci..
[64] S. Lupker,et al. Masked inhibitory priming in english: evidence for lexical inhibition. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[65] Jonathan Grainger,et al. Temporal Integration of Information in Orthographic Priming , 1999 .
[66] Max Coltheart,et al. Paying attention to reading errors in acquired dyslexia , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[67] Stephen Grossberg,et al. A Theory of Human Memory: Self-Organization and Performance of Sensory-Motor Codes, Maps, and Plans , 1982 .
[68] K. Forster,et al. REPETITION PRIMING AND FREQUENCY ATTENUATION IN LEXICAL ACCESS , 1984 .
[69] Colin J. Davis,et al. The self-organising lexical acquisition and recognition (SOLAR) model of visual word recognition. , 2001 .
[70] S. Liversedge,et al. Neighborhood Effects Using a Partial Priming Methodology: Guessing or Activation? , 1998 .
[71] R W Schvaneveldt,et al. An activation--verification model for letter and word recognition: the word-superiority effect. , 1982, Psychological review.
[72] Jeffrey S Bowers,et al. Challenging the widespread assumption that connectionism and distributed representations go hand-in-hand , 2002, Cognitive Psychology.
[73] A. Jacobs,et al. Optimal viewing position effect in word recognition: A challenge to current theory. , 1992 .
[74] Jeffrey S Bowers,et al. What do letter migration errors reveal about letter position coding in visual word recognition? , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[75] Ton Dijkstra,et al. Shared neighborhood effects in masked orthographic priming , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[76] Andrew W. Ellis,et al. "Neglect dyslexia" and the early visual processing of letters in words and nonwords , 1995 .
[77] Colin J. Davis,et al. Factors underlying masked priming effects in competitive network models of visual word recognition , 2003 .
[78] Max Coltheart,et al. Access to the internal lexicon , 1977 .
[79] M Coltheart,et al. DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.
[80] D. Norris,et al. The primacy model: a new model of immediate serial recall. , 1998, Psychological review.
[81] Neighborhood frequency does affect performance in the Reicher task: encoding or decision? , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[82] S. Lupker,et al. Does jugde activate COURT? Transposed-letter similarity effects in masked associative priming , 2003, Memory & cognition.
[83] Patrick Bonin,et al. Mental lexicon : "some words to talk about words" , 2004 .