Contrasting five different theories of letter position coding: evidence from orthographic similarity effects.

Five theories of how letter position is coded are contrasted: position-specific slot-coding, Wickelcoding, open-bigram coding (discrete and continuous), and spatial coding. These theories make different predictions regarding the relative similarity of three different types of pairs of letter strings: substitution neighbors, neighbors-once-removed, and double-substitution neighbors. In Experiment 1, we used an illusory word paradigm and found that neighbor-once-removed similarity contexts resulted in fewer illusory word reports than substitution neighbors but more illusory words than double-substitution neighbors. In Experiments 2 and 3, we used a masked form priming technique with a lexical-decision task. The pattern of facilitation was as predicted by spatial coding but was incompatible with slot-coding, Wickelcoding, and both versions of open-bigram coding. These results provide further support for the SOLAR (self-organizing lexical aquisition and recognition) model of visual word identification.

[1]  Manuel Perea,et al.  Transposed-Letter Confusability Effects in Masked Form Priming , 2003 .

[2]  S. Andrews The effect of orthographic similarity on lexical retrieval: Resolving neighborhood conflicts , 1997 .

[3]  K. Paap,et al.  Neighborhood frequency does affect performance in the Reicher task: encoding or decision? , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  Ken N. Seergobin,et al.  On the association between connectionism and data: Are a few words necessary? , 1990 .

[5]  C. Whitney How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective literature review , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[6]  K I Forster,et al.  The prime lexicality effect: form-priming as a function of prime awareness, lexical status, and discrimination difficulty. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[7]  M. Zorzi,et al.  Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual-process model. , 1998 .

[8]  M Taft,et al.  Bodies, antibodies, and neighborhood-density effects in masked form priming. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[9]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  The Time Course of Orthographic and Phonological Code Activation , 2006 .

[10]  W. Estes,et al.  Serial position functions for letter identification at brief and extended exposure durations , 1976 .

[11]  M Brysbaert,et al.  Prelexical phonological coding of visual words in Dutch: Automatic after all , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[12]  J. Requin Attention and Performance VII , 1980 .

[13]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .

[14]  K. Rayner Eye Guidance in Reading: Fixation Locations within Words , 1979, Perception.

[15]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.

[16]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Lesioning an attractor network: investigations of acquired dyslexia , 1991 .

[17]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: insights from connectionist models. , 1999, Psychological review.

[18]  J Grainger,et al.  Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: a multiple read-out model. , 1996, Psychological review.

[19]  A. Treisman,et al.  Illusory words: the roles of attention and of top-down constraints in conjoining letters to form words. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  M. Mozer,et al.  Perceptual interactions in two-word displays: familiarity and similarity effects. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  T. M. Ellison,et al.  Eye-fixation behavior, lexical storage, and visual word recognition in a split processing model. , 2000, Psychological review.

[22]  M Perea,et al.  Repetition and form priming interact with neighborhood density at a brief stimulus onset asynchrony , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[23]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Localist connectionist approaches to human cognition , 1998 .

[24]  Kenneth I Forster,et al.  DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy , 2003, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[25]  J. Grainger,et al.  Letter position coding in printed word perception: Effects of repeated and transposed letters , 2004 .

[26]  S. M. Chambers Letter and order information in lexical access , 1979 .

[27]  S. Andrews Lexical Retrieval and Selection Processes: Effects of Transposed-Letter Confusability , 1996 .

[28]  Michael C. Mozer,et al.  Perception of multiple objects - a connectionist approach , 1991, Neural network modeling and connectionism.

[29]  Padraic Monaghan,et al.  The Computational Exploration of Visual Word Recognition in a Split Model , 2001, Neural Computation.

[30]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[31]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The Use of Abstract Graphemic Information in Lexical Access , 1981 .

[32]  Sally Andrews,et al.  Interactive Activation Accounts of Morphological Decomposition: Finding the Trap in Mousetrap? , 1999, Brain and Language.

[33]  Sachiko Kinoshita,et al.  Masked priming : the state of the art , 2003 .

[34]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Modeling letter position coding in printed word perception , 2004 .

[35]  Randolph G. Bias,et al.  Word recognition inside out and outside in , 1981 .

[36]  J. Grainger,et al.  Does the huamn mnid raed wrods as a wlohe? , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[37]  A. Jacobs,et al.  On the role of competing word units in visual word recognition: The neighborhood frequency effect , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[38]  Philip T Quinlan,et al.  Orthographic processing in visual word identification , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[39]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Letter position information and printed word perception: the relative-position priming constraint. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  K I Forster,et al.  The potential for experimenter bias effects in word recognition experiments , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[41]  Michael E. J. Masson,et al.  A Retrospective View of Masked Priming : Toward a Unified Account of Masked and Long-Term Repetition Priming , 2003 .

[42]  C. Davis,et al.  More words in the neighborhood: Interference in lexical decision due to deletion neighbors , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[43]  J. Grainger,et al.  Priming word recognition with orthographic neighbors: effects of relative prime-target frequency. , 1990 .

[44]  W A Wickelgren,et al.  Auditory or articulatory coding in verbal short-term memory. , 1969, Psychological review.

[45]  K. Forster,et al.  Masked priming with graphemically related forms: Repetition or partial activation? , 1987 .

[46]  J Grainger,et al.  The role of letter identity and letter position in orthographic priming , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[47]  M. Taft,et al.  Lack of Phonological Mediation in a Semantic Categorization Task , 1998 .

[48]  C. Perfetti,et al.  Phonemic activation during the first 40 ms of word identification: Evidence from backward masking and priming , 1991 .

[49]  Manuel Perea,et al.  Orthographic Neighbours are not all Equal: Evidence using an Identification Technique , 1998, Language and Cognitive Processes.

[50]  M. Brysbaert,et al.  Neighborhood-frequency effects when primes and targets are of different lengths , 2000, Psychological research.

[51]  Michael Garman,et al.  Psycholinguistics: Accessing the mental lexicon , 1990 .

[52]  C. Davis N-Watch: A program for deriving neighborhood size and other psycholinguistic statistics , 2005, Behavior research methods.

[53]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Neighborhood frequency effects and letter visibility in visual word recognition , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[54]  Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al.  Identification, localization, and “iconic memory”: An evaluation of the bar-probe task , 1981, Memory & cognition.

[55]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .

[56]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Phonology and orthography in visual word recognition , 1991 .

[57]  K. Forster Form-priming with masked primes: The best match hypothesis. , 1987 .

[58]  B. Bergum,et al.  Attention and Performance VI , 1978 .

[59]  R S Berndt,et al.  A new model of letter string encoding: simulating right neglect dyslexia. , 1999, Progress in brain research.

[60]  P. Zwitserlood,et al.  Morphological and orthographic similarity in visual word recognition. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[61]  James L. McClelland,et al.  On learning the past-tenses of English verbs: implicit rules or parallel distributed processing , 1986 .

[62]  J. Bowers,et al.  Automatic semantic activation of embedded words: Is there a hat in that? , 2005 .

[63]  MICHAEL P. A. PAGE Modelling the Perception of Musical Sequences with Self-organizing Neural Networks , 1994, Connect. Sci..

[64]  S. Lupker,et al.  Masked inhibitory priming in english: evidence for lexical inhibition. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[65]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Temporal Integration of Information in Orthographic Priming , 1999 .

[66]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Paying attention to reading errors in acquired dyslexia , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[67]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  A Theory of Human Memory: Self-Organization and Performance of Sensory-Motor Codes, Maps, and Plans , 1982 .

[68]  K. Forster,et al.  REPETITION PRIMING AND FREQUENCY ATTENUATION IN LEXICAL ACCESS , 1984 .

[69]  Colin J. Davis,et al.  The self-organising lexical acquisition and recognition (SOLAR) model of visual word recognition. , 2001 .

[70]  S. Liversedge,et al.  Neighborhood Effects Using a Partial Priming Methodology: Guessing or Activation? , 1998 .

[71]  R W Schvaneveldt,et al.  An activation--verification model for letter and word recognition: the word-superiority effect. , 1982, Psychological review.

[72]  Jeffrey S Bowers,et al.  Challenging the widespread assumption that connectionism and distributed representations go hand-in-hand , 2002, Cognitive Psychology.

[73]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Optimal viewing position effect in word recognition: A challenge to current theory. , 1992 .

[74]  Jeffrey S Bowers,et al.  What do letter migration errors reveal about letter position coding in visual word recognition? , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[75]  Ton Dijkstra,et al.  Shared neighborhood effects in masked orthographic priming , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[76]  Andrew W. Ellis,et al.  "Neglect dyslexia" and the early visual processing of letters in words and nonwords , 1995 .

[77]  Colin J. Davis,et al.  Factors underlying masked priming effects in competitive network models of visual word recognition , 2003 .

[78]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Access to the internal lexicon , 1977 .

[79]  M Coltheart,et al.  DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.

[80]  D. Norris,et al.  The primacy model: a new model of immediate serial recall. , 1998, Psychological review.

[81]  Neighborhood frequency does affect performance in the Reicher task: encoding or decision? , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[82]  S. Lupker,et al.  Does jugde activate COURT? Transposed-letter similarity effects in masked associative priming , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[83]  Patrick Bonin,et al.  Mental lexicon : "some words to talk about words" , 2004 .