Attention capture by eye of origin singletons even without awareness--a hallmark of a bottom-up saliency map in the primary visual cortex.

Human observers are typically unaware of the eye of origin of visual inputs. This study shows that an eye of origin or ocular singleton, e.g., an item in the left eye among background items in the right eye, can nevertheless attract attention automatically. Observers searched for a uniquely oriented bar, i.e., an orientation singleton, in a background of horizontal bars. Their reports of the tilt direction of the search target in a brief (200 ms) display were more accurate in a dichoptic congruent (DC) condition, when the target was also an ocular singleton, than in a monocular (M) condition, when all bars were presented to the same single eye, or a dichoptic incongruent (DI) condition, when an ocular singleton was a background bar. The better performance in DC did not depend on the ability of the observers to report the presence of an ocular singleton by making forced choices in the same stimuli (though without the orientation singleton). This suggests that the ocular singleton exogenously cued attention to its location, facilitating the identification of the tilt singleton in the DC condition. When the search display persisted without being masked, observers' reaction times (RTs) for reporting the location of the search target were shorter in the DC, and longer in the DI, than the M condition, regardless of whether the observers were aware that different conditions existed. In an analogous design, similar RT patterns were observed for the task of finding an orientation contrast texture border. These results suggest that in typical trials, attention was more quickly attracted to or initially distracted from the target in the DC or DI condition, respectively. Hence, an ocular singleton, though elusive to awareness, can effectively compete for attention with an orientation singleton (tilted 20 or 50 degrees from background bars in the current study). Similarly, it can also make a difficult visual search easier by diminishing the set size effect. Since monocular neurons with the eye of origin information are abundant in the primary visual cortex (V1) and scarce in other cortical areas, and since visual awareness is believed to be absent or weaker in V1 than in other cortical areas, our results provide a hallmark of the role of V1 in creating a bottom-up saliency map to guide attentional selection.

[1]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  S. Zeki Uniformity and diversity of structure and function in rhesus monkey prestriate visual cortex. , 1978, The Journal of physiology.

[3]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[4]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  B. Julesz Textons, the elements of texture perception, and their interactions , 1981, Nature.

[6]  J. Jonides Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind's eye's movement , 1981 .

[7]  B. Bergum,et al.  Attention and performance IX , 1982 .

[8]  J. Lund,et al.  Intrinsic laminar lattice connections in primate visual cortex , 1983, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[9]  T. Wiesel,et al.  Clustered intrinsic connections in cat visual cortex , 1983, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  S Ullman,et al.  Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[11]  B Julesz,et al.  "Where" and "what" in vision. , 1985, Science.

[12]  J. Allman,et al.  Stimulus specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field: neurophysiological mechanisms for local-global comparisons in visual neurons. , 1985, Annual review of neuroscience.

[13]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Processing of color, form and disparity information in visual areas VP and V2 of ventral extrastriate cortex in the macaque monkey , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[14]  DH Hubel,et al.  Segregation of form, color, and stereopsis in primate area 18 , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  H Pashler,et al.  Cross-dimensional interaction and texture segregation , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[16]  Susan L. Franzel,et al.  Binocularity and visual search , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[17]  J. Duncan,et al.  Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.

[18]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention , 1989, Vision Research.

[19]  Susan L. Franzel,et al.  Guided search: an alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  J A Solomon,et al.  Texture interactions determine perceived contrast , 1989, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  H. C. Nothdurft,et al.  Texture segmentation and pop-out from orientation contrast , 1991, Vision Research.

[23]  D. Foster,et al.  Asymmetries in oriented-line detection indicate two orthogonal filters in early vision , 1991, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[24]  H. Nothdurft Feature analysis and the role of similarity in preattentive vision , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[25]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Neuronal responses to static texture patterns in area V1 of the alert macaque monkey. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  H. Nothdurft The role of features in preattentive vision: Comparison of orientation, motion and color cues , 1993, Vision Research.

[27]  C. Li,et al.  Extensive integration field beyond the classical receptive field of cat's striate cortical neurons--classification and tuning properties. , 1994, Vision research.

[28]  I. Ohzawa,et al.  Length and width tuning of neurons in the cat's primary visual cortex. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[30]  Jochen Braun,et al.  Blindsight in normal observers , 1995, Nature.

[31]  H. Jones,et al.  Visual cortical mechanisms detecting focal orientation discontinuities , 1995, Nature.

[32]  C. Koch,et al.  Are we aware of neural activity in primary visual cortex? , 1995, Nature.

[33]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[34]  Victor A. F. Lamme The neurophysiology of figure-ground segregation in primary visual cortex , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[35]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Attentional resolution and the locus of visual awareness , 1996, Nature.

[36]  K R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Processing of color, form, and motion in macaque area V2 , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.

[37]  David A. Leopold,et al.  What is rivalling during binocular rivalry? , 1996, Nature.

[38]  N. Logothetis,et al.  What is rivalling during binocular rivalry , 1996 .

[39]  H. Tamura,et al.  Less Segregated Processing of Visual Information in V2 than in V1 of the Monkey Visual Cortex , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[40]  S. Kastner,et al.  Neuronal Correlates of Pop-out in Cat Striate Cortex , 1997, Vision Research.

[41]  M. Georgeson Vision and Action: You Ain'T Seen Nothin' Yet …. 1. Paradigms and Puzzles: Hindsight in Normal Observers , 1997 .

[42]  M. Morgan,et al.  Blindsight in normal subjects? , 1997, Nature.

[43]  D. Snodderly,et al.  A Dissociation Between Brain Activity and Perception: Chromatically Opponent Cortical Neurons Signal Chromatic Flicker that is not Perceived , 1997, Vision Research.

[44]  D. Fitzpatrick,et al.  Orientation Selectivity and the Arrangement of Horizontal Connections in Tree Shrew Striate Cortex , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  J. B. Levitt,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in macaque area V3. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Hierarchical Model of Binocular Rivalry , 1998, Neural Computation.

[47]  C. Erkelens,et al.  Adjustment of fixation duration in visual search , 1998, Vision Research.

[48]  Todd S. Horowitz,et al.  Visual search has no memory , 1998, Nature.

[49]  D. Ffytche,et al.  The anatomy of conscious vision: an fMRI study of visual hallucinations , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[50]  M. Goldberg,et al.  The representation of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex , 1998, Nature.

[51]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Response modulation by texture surround in primate area V1: Correlates of “popout” under anesthesia , 1999, Visual Neuroscience.

[52]  S. Kastner,et al.  Neuronal responses to orientation and motion contrast in cat striate cortex , 1999, Visual Neuroscience.

[53]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural selection and control of visually guided eye movements. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.

[54]  Z Li,et al.  Visual segmentation by contextual influences via intra-cortical interactions in the primary visual cortex. , 1999, Network.

[55]  Z Li,et al.  Contextual influences in V1 as a basis for pop out and asymmetry in visual search. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[56]  Z Li,et al.  Pre-attentive segmentation in the primary visual cortex. , 1998, Spatial vision.

[57]  B. Rosen,et al.  Mechanisms of migraine aura revealed by functional MRI in human visual cortex , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[58]  C. Koch,et al.  Computational modelling of visual attention , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[59]  H. Müller,et al.  Visual search for dimensionally redundant pop-out targets: Evidence for parallel-coactive processing of dimensions , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[60]  Sheng He,et al.  Orientation-selective adaptation and tilt after-effect from invisible patterns , 2001, Nature.

[61]  H. Jones,et al.  Surround suppression in primate V1. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[62]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Human brain activity during spontaneously reversing perception of ambiguous figures , 1998, 5th IEEE EMBS International Summer School on Biomedical Imaging, 2002..

[63]  Alexander Grunewald,et al.  Neural Correlates of Structure-from-Motion Perception in Macaque V1 and MT , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[64]  Zhaoping Li A saliency map in primary visual cortex , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[65]  E. T. Bullmore,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of synesthesia: activation of V4/V8 by spoken words , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[66]  E. J. Tehovnik,et al.  Saccadic eye movements evoked by microstimulation of striate cortex , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[67]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Representation of Color Stimuli in Awake Macaque Primary Visual Cortex , 2003, Neuron.

[68]  Anthony Lambert,et al.  The influence of a salience distinction between bilateral cues on the latency of target-detection saccades. , 2003, British journal of psychology.

[69]  Frank Tong,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience: Primary visual cortex and visual awareness , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[70]  R. Desimone,et al.  Interacting Roles of Attention and Visual Salience in V4 , 2003, Neuron.

[71]  Jillian H. Fecteau,et al.  Neural correlates of the automatic and goal-driven biases in orienting spatial attention. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[72]  Susana Martinez-Conde,et al.  Dichoptic Visual Masking Reveals that Early Binocular Neurons Exhibit Weak Interocular Suppression: Implications for Binocular Vision and Visual Awareness , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[73]  Joshua A Solomon,et al.  The effect of spatial cues on visual sensitivity , 2004, Vision Research.

[74]  M. Posner The Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention , 2020 .

[75]  P. Lennie,et al.  Early and Late Mechanisms of Surround Suppression in Striate Cortex of Macaque , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[76]  John K. Tsotsos,et al.  Neurobiology of Attention , 2005 .

[77]  Gregory D Horwitz,et al.  Paucity of chromatic linear motion detectors in macaque V1. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[78]  S. Kastner,et al.  Stimulus context modulates competition in human extrastriate cortex , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[79]  Eds L. Itti,et al.  The primary visual cortex creates a bottom-up saliency map , 2005 .

[80]  Adrian John,et al.  Monocular texture segmentation and proto-rivalry , 2006, Vision Research.

[81]  Wieske van Zoest,et al.  Saccadic target selection as a function of time. , 2006, Spatial vision.

[82]  Fang Fang,et al.  A gender- and sexual orientation-dependent spatial attentional effect of invisible images , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[83]  Peter Dayan,et al.  Pre-attentive visual selection , 2006, Neural Networks.

[84]  Li Zhaoping,et al.  Theoretical understanding of the early visual processes by data compression and data selection , 2006, Network.

[85]  Barry B. Lee,et al.  Suppressive Surrounds and Contrast Gain in Magnocellular-Pathway Retinal Ganglion Cells of Macaque , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[86]  L. Zhaoping,et al.  A theory of a saliency map in primary visual cortex (V1) tested by psychophysics of colour–orientation interference in texture segmentation , 2006 .

[87]  Ke Zhou,et al.  Human visual cortex responds to invisible chromatic flicker , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[88]  Nathalie Guyader,et al.  Interference with Bottom-Up Feature Detection by Higher-Level Object Recognition , 2007, Current Biology.

[89]  Li Zhaoping Popout by unique eye of origin: A fingerprint of the role of primary visual cortex in bottom-up saliency , 2007 .

[90]  Zhaoping Li,et al.  Psychophysical Tests of the Hypothesis of a Bottom-Up Saliency Map in Primary Visual Cortex , 2007, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[91]  Zhaoping Li,et al.  Feature-specific interactions in salience from combined feature contrasts: evidence for a bottom-up saliency map in V1. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[92]  C. Koch,et al.  Attention and consciousness: two distinct brain processes , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[93]  Li Jingling,et al.  Change Detection is Easier at Texture Border Bars When They are Parallel to the Border: Evidence for V1 Mechanisms of Bottom-up Salience , 2008, Perception.

[94]  W. Martin Usrey,et al.  Origin and Dynamics of Extraclassical Suppression in the Lateral Geniculate Nucleus of the Macaque Monkey , 2008, Neuron.