Consistency and regularity in past-tense verb generation in healthy ageing, Alzheimer's disease, and semantic dementia

Older adults, individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer's type (DAT), and individuals with semantic dementia (SD) produced the past tense of verbs based on present-tense carrier sentences (e.g., Everyday I ding the bell. Yesterday I_____the bell). Both regularity (i.e., whether or not -ed is used for the past tense) and consistency (i.e., the degree to which verbs of similar orthography and phonology in the present tense have similar past tenses to the target) were manipulated. Participants received regular consistent (e.g., land–landed), regular inconsistent (e.g., weed–weeded), irregular consistent (e.g., sting–stung), and irregular inconsistent (e.g., light–lit) verbs. The dependent measures were overall accuracy rates and error rate types (e.g., regularizations, analogies, and other errors). Both consistency and regularity influenced performance. In addition, individuals with DAT showed a disproportionate deficit for inconsistent verbs associated with a high summed frequency of enemies, whereas SD individuals produced disproportionate breakdowns in performance on regular inconsistent, irregular consistent, and irregular inconsistent verbs. These results are consistent with the perspective that semantic/lexical processes are involved in processing the past tense of both irregular verbs and regular inconsistent verbs, and that attention is used to select appropriate responses and control inappropriate responses.

[1]  J. Raven Coloured progressive matrices : sets A, Ab, B , 1956 .

[2]  H. H. Clark The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. , 1973 .

[3]  J. Baron,et al.  Use of orthographic and word-specific knowledge in reading words aloud. , 1976 .

[4]  M. Folstein,et al.  Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease , 1984, Neurology.

[5]  J. Rogin,et al.  Peripheral sensory neuropathy and cisplatin chemotherapy , 1984, Neurology.

[6]  James L. McClelland,et al.  On learning the past-tenses of English verbs: implicit rules or parallel distributed processing , 1986 .

[7]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Psychological and biological models , 1986 .

[8]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .

[9]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Consistency effects in the generation of past tense morphology , 1990 .

[10]  David A. Balota,et al.  Semantic priming effects, lexical repetition effects, and contextual disambiguation effects in healthy aged individuals and individuals with senile dementia of the Alzheimer type , 1991, Brain and Language.

[11]  A. Burns Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease , 1991 .

[12]  M. Sliwinski,et al.  Development and validation of a model for estimating premorbid verbal intelligence in the elderly. , 1991, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[13]  J. Hodges,et al.  Semantic dementia. Progressive fluent aphasia with temporal lobe atrophy. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[14]  D. Balota,et al.  A dissociation of frequency and regularity effects in pronunciation performance across young adults, older adults, and individuals with senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. , 1993 .

[15]  S. Pinker The Language Instinct , 1994 .

[16]  B. Ober,et al.  Semantic priming in Alzheimer's disease: Meta-analysis and theoretical evaluation , 1995 .

[17]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.

[18]  D. Balota,et al.  Stroop performance in healthy younger and older adults and in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer's type. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  Debra Jared,et al.  Spelling-Sound Consistency Affects the Naming of High-Frequency Words , 1997 .

[20]  D. Wechsler Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale , 2021, Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science.

[21]  S. Pinker,et al.  A Neural Dissociation within Language: Evidence that the Mental Dictionary Is Part of Declarative Memory, and that Grammatical Rules Are Processed by the Procedural System , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  J. Price,et al.  Clinicopathologic studies in cognitively healthy aging and Alzheimer's disease: relation of histologic markers to dementia severity, age, sex, and apolipoprotein E genotype. , 1998, Archives of neurology.

[23]  R. Faber,et al.  Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. , 1999, Neurology.

[24]  M. Ullman Acceptability Ratings of Regular and Irregular Past-tense Forms: Evidence for a Dual-system Model of Language from Word Frequency and Phonological Neighbourhood Effects , 1999 .

[25]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Impairments in verb morphology after brain injury: a connectionist model. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  J. Hodges,et al.  Non-verbal semantic impairment in semantic dementia , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  J. Algeo Words and Rules: The Ingredients of Language , 2000 .

[28]  Michael J Cortese,et al.  Regularity effects in word naming: What are they? , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[29]  J. Hodges,et al.  Differentiating frontal and temporal variant frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimer’s disease , 2000, Neurology.

[30]  A. Kertesz,et al.  Phonologically Related Lexical Repetition Disorder: A Case Study , 2001, Brain and Language.

[31]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Deficits in irregular past-tense verb morphology associated with degraded semantic knowledge , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  M Coltheart,et al.  DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.

[33]  B. Sahakian,et al.  Temporal lobe rating scale: application to Alzheimer's disease and frontotemporal dementia , 2001, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[34]  Takashi Hanakawa,et al.  Modulation of the Visual Word Retrieval System in Writing: A Functional MRI Study on the Japanese Orthographies , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  Dissociations in Processing Past Tense Morphology: Neuropathology and Behavioral Studies , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[36]  Jenny R. Saffran,et al.  Does Grammar Start Where Statistics Stop? , 2002, Science.

[37]  James L. McClelland,et al.  ‘Words or Rules’ cannot exploit the regularity in exceptions , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  D. Jared Spelling-Sound Consistency and Regularity Effects in Word Naming , 2002 .

[39]  Maryellen C. MacDonald,et al.  Language learning and innateness: Some implications of Compounds Research , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[40]  Michele Miozzo,et al.  On the processing of regular and irregular forms of verbs and nouns: evidence from neuropsychology , 2003, Cognition.

[41]  Norbert Schuff,et al.  Cinguloparietal atrophy distinguishes Alzheimer disease from semantic dementia. , 2003, Archives of neurology.

[42]  Michael J Cortese,et al.  Spelling via semantics and phonology: exploring the effects of age, Alzheimer’s disease, and primary semantic impairment , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Deficits in phonology and past-tense morphology: What's the connection? , 2003 .

[44]  P Garrard,et al.  Dissociation of Lexical Syntax and Semantics: Evidence from Focal Cortical Degeneration , 2004, Neurocase.

[45]  M. Ullman Contributions of memory circuits to language: the declarative/procedural model , 2004, Cognition.

[46]  David Wechsler,et al.  Wechsler Memory scale. , 2005 .

[47]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  Differing neuropsychological and neuroanatomical correlates of abnormal reading in early-stage semantic dementia and dementia of the Alzheimer type , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Cognitive Neuropsychology , 2014, Scholarpedia.