Neural correlates of orthographic and phonological consistency effects in children

The objective of this study was to examine the neural correlates of phonological inconsistency (relationship of spelling to sound) and orthographic inconsistency (relationship of sound to spelling) in visual word processing using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Children (9‐ to 15‐year‐old) performed a rhyming and spelling task in which two words were presented sequentially in the visual modality. Consistent with previous studies in adults, higher phonological inconsistency was associated with greater activation in several regions including left inferior frontal gyrus and medial frontal gyrus/anterior cingulate cortex. We additionally demonstrated an effect of orthographic inconsistency in these same areas, suggesting that these regions are involved in the integration of orthographic and phonological information and, with respect to the medial frontal/anterior cingulate, greater demands on executive function. Higher phonological and orthographic consistency was associated with greater activation in precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex, the putative steady state system active during resting, suggesting lower demands on cognitive resources for consistent items. Both consistency effects were larger for the rhyming compared with the spelling task suggesting greater demands of integrating spelling and sound in the former task. Finally, accuracy on the rhyming task was negatively correlated with the consistency effect in left fusiform gyrus. In particular, this region showed insensitivity to consistency in low performers, sensitivity to inconsistency (higher activity) in moderate performers, and sensitivity to inconsistency (high activation) and to consistency (deactivation). In general, these results show that the influence of spelling–sound (and sound–spelling) correspondences on processing in fusiform gyrus develops as a function of skill. Hum Brain Mapp, 2008. © 2007 Wiley‐Liss, Inc.

[1]  Barbara J. Wendling,et al.  Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Achievement. , 2009 .

[2]  Rebecca Treiman,et al.  Feedback-consistency effects in single-word reading: Brett Kessler, Rebecca Treiman, and John , 2007 .

[3]  R. Treiman,et al.  Anticipatory conditioning of spelling-to-sound translation☆ , 2007 .

[4]  James R. Booth,et al.  Weaker top–down modulation from the left inferior frontal gyrus in children , 2006, NeuroImage.

[5]  L.M. Balsamo,et al.  Language lateralization and the role of the fusiform gyrus in semantic processing in young children , 2006, NeuroImage.

[6]  Bernard Mazoyer,et al.  Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: Phonology, semantics, and sentence processing , 2006, NeuroImage.

[7]  Giuseppe Sartori,et al.  Semantic relevance explains category effects in medial fusiform gyri , 2006, NeuroImage.

[8]  Whitney Tabor,et al.  Behavioral and neurobiological effects of printed word repetition in lexical decision and naming , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  Michael S. Beauchamp,et al.  Automatic Priming of Semantically Related Words Reduces Activity in the Fusiform Gyrus , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  Jeffrey R. Binder,et al.  Some neurophysiological constraints on models of word naming , 2005, NeuroImage.

[11]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  Cameron S Carter,et al.  Cognitive control involved in overcoming prepotent response tendencies and switching between tasks. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[13]  M. Mesulam,et al.  Shifts of Effective Connectivity within a Language Network during Rhyming and Spelling , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  Dominic W. Massaro,et al.  The magic of reading: Too many influences for quick and easy explanations. , 2005 .

[15]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Distinct unimodal and multimodal regions for word processing in the left temporal cortex , 2004, NeuroImage.

[16]  Isabel Lacruz,et al.  Feedforward and Feedback Consistency Effects for High- and Low-Frequency Words in Lexical Decision and Naming , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[17]  A. Friederici,et al.  Brain Correlates of Language Learning: The Neuronal Dissociation of Rule-Based versus Similarity-Based Learning , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  Qian Luo,et al.  fMRI evidence for the automatic phonological activation of briefly presented words. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[19]  Masato Taira,et al.  Reading in a Regular Orthography: An fMRI Study Investigating the Role of Visual Familiarity , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  J B Poline,et al.  Letter Binding and Invariant Recognition of Masked Words , 2004, Psychological science.

[21]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[22]  Karsten Müller,et al.  Distinct brain representations for early and late learned words , 2003, NeuroImage.

[23]  Wilma Koutstaal,et al.  Neural mechanisms of visual object priming: evidence for perceptual and semantic distinctions in fusiform cortex , 2003, NeuroImage.

[24]  Joel R. Meyer,et al.  Relation between brain activation and lexical performance , 2003, Human brain mapping.

[25]  E. T. Possing,et al.  Neural Correlates of Lexical Access during Visual Word Recognition , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  Tzu-Chen Yeh,et al.  Frequency effects of Chinese character processing in the brain: an event-related fMRI study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[27]  Rebecca Treiman,et al.  Phonetic Biases in Voice Key Response Time Measurements , 2002 .

[28]  James R. Booth,et al.  Functional Anatomy of Intra- and Cross-Modal Lexical Tasks , 2002, NeuroImage.

[29]  J. Mitchell Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing , 2001 .

[30]  P T Fox,et al.  An fMRI study with written Chinese , 2001, Neuroreport.

[31]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[32]  S. Petersen,et al.  Effects of Lexicality, Frequency, and Spelling-to-Sound Consistency on the Functional Anatomy of Reading , 1999, Neuron.

[33]  J. Desmond,et al.  Functional Specialization for Semantic and Phonological Processing in the Left Inferior Prefrontal Cortex , 1999, NeuroImage.

[34]  B MacWhinney,et al.  Quick, automatic, and general activation of orthographic and phonological representations in young readers. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[35]  M. Raichle,et al.  The neural correlates of consciousness: an analysis of cognitive skill learning. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[36]  A M Dale,et al.  Randomized event‐related experimental designs allow for extremely rapid presentation rates using functional MRI , 1998, Neuroreport.

[37]  R. Peereman,et al.  Is perception a two-way street ?: The case of feedback consistency in visual word recognition , 1998 .

[38]  J. Ziegler,et al.  The Feedback Consistency Effect in Lexical Decision and Naming , 1997 .

[39]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  Debra Jared,et al.  Spelling-Sound Consistency Affects the Naming of High-Frequency Words , 1997 .

[41]  G. Stone,et al.  Perception Is a Two-Way Street: Feedforward and Feedback Phonology in Visual Word Recognition ☆ , 1997 .

[42]  M. Mintun,et al.  Regional cerebral blood flow during word and nonword reading , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[43]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.

[44]  S. Petersen,et al.  Practice-related changes in human brain functional anatomy during nonmotor learning. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[45]  C. Perfetti,et al.  Learning to read : basic research and its implications , 1993 .

[46]  Morag Stuart,et al.  Patterns of reading and spelling in 10-year-old children related to prereading phonological abilities , 1992 .

[47]  C. Perfetti The Representation Problem in Reading Acquisition , 1992 .

[48]  V. Coltheart,et al.  Some Bodies are Easier to Read: The Effect of Consistency and Regularity on Children's Reading , 1991 .

[49]  S. Zecker,et al.  The orthographic code: Developmental trends in reading-disabled and normally-achieving children , 1991, Annals of dyslexia.

[50]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  The basis of consistency effects in word naming , 1990 .

[51]  Charles A. Perfetti,et al.  Automatic (prelexical) phonetic activation in silent word reading: Evidence from backward masking*1 , 1988 .

[52]  L. Ehri Learning to Read and Spell Words , 1987 .

[53]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  When does irregular spelling or pronunciation influence word recognition , 1984 .

[54]  G. Waters,et al.  Children’s and adults’ use of spelling-sound information in three reading tasks , 1984, Memory & cognition.

[55]  Dolores Perin,et al.  Phonemic segmentation and spelling , 1983 .

[56]  William E. Tunmer,et al.  The effects of digraphs and pseudowords on phonemic segmentation in young children , 1982, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[57]  D. Boswell,et al.  The abstraction and recognition of prototypes by children and adults. , 1982, Child development.

[58]  Linnea C. Ehri,et al.  The influence of orthography on readers' conceptualization of the phonemic structure of words , 1980, Applied Psycholinguistics.