Demography and Cultural Evolution: How Adaptive Cultural Processes Can Produce Maladaptive Losses—The Tasmanian Case

A combination of archeological and ethnohistorical evidence indicates that, over an approximately 8,000-year period, from the beginning of the Holocene until European explorers began arriving in the eighteenth century, the societies of Tasmania lost a series of valuable skills and technologies. These likely included bone tools, cold-weather clothing, hafted tools, nets, fishing spears, barbed spears, spear-throwers, and boomerangs. To address this puzzle, and the more general question of how human cognition and social interaction can generate both adaptive cultural evolution and maladaptive losses of culturally acquired skills, this paper constructs a formal model of cultural evolution rooted in the cognitive details of human social learning and inference. The analytical results specify the conditions for differing rates of adaptive cultural evolution, and reveal regimes that will produce maladaptive losses of particular kinds of skills and related technologies. More specifically, the results suggest that the relatively sudden reduction in the effective population size (the size of the interacting pool of social learners) that occurred with the rising ocean levels at the end of the last glacial epoch, which cut Tasmania off from the rest of Australia for the ensuing ten millennia, could have initiated a cultural evolutionary process that (1) kept stable or even improved relatively simple technological skills, and (2) produced an increasing deterioration of more complex skills leading to the complete disappearance of some technologies and practices. This pattern is consistent with the empirical record in Tasmania. Beyond this case, I speculate on the applicability of the model to understanding the variability in rates of adaptive cultural evolution.

[1]  N. Plomley Friendly mission : the Tasmanian journals and papers of George Augustus Robinson, 1829-1834 , 1966 .

[2]  George R. Price,et al.  Selection and Covariance , 1970, Nature.

[3]  G. Price,et al.  Extension of covariance selection mathematics , 1972, Annals of human genetics.

[4]  Wendell H. Oswalt Habitat and technology;: The evolution of hunting , 1972 .

[5]  S. Bowdler Pleistocene date for man in Tasmania , 1974, Nature.

[6]  Wendell H. Oswalt,et al.  An anthropological analysis of food-getting technology , 1976 .

[7]  G. Clark,et al.  Problems in economic and social archaeology , 1976 .

[8]  S. K. Lothrop The Indians of Tierra Del Fuego , 1976 .

[9]  J. Diamond Colonization cycles in man and beast. , 1977, World archaeology.

[10]  Jared M. Diamond,et al.  The Tasmanians: the longest isolation, the simplest technology , 1978, Nature.

[11]  R. V. S. Wright,et al.  Stone Tools As Cultural Markers: Change, Evolution and Complexity , 1978 .

[12]  Rl Vanderwal Adaptive technology in Southwest Tasmania , 1978 .

[13]  Lyndall Ryan The Aboriginal Tasmanians , 1981 .

[14]  M. Chibnik The Evolution of Cultural Rules , 1981, Journal of Anthropological Research.

[15]  M. Feldman,et al.  A study of cultural transmission in Taiwan , 1982 .

[16]  James F. O'Connell,et al.  A prehistory of Australia, New Guinea, and Sahul , 1982 .

[17]  L. Binford,et al.  In pursuit of the past : decoding the archaeological record : with a new afterword , 1983 .

[18]  Barry S. Hewlett,et al.  Cultural Transmission Among Aka Pygmies , 1986 .

[19]  W. McGrew,et al.  Tools to Get Food: The Subsistants of Tasmanian Aborigines and Tanzanian Chimpanzees Compared , 1987, Journal of Anthropological Research.

[20]  Jim Allen,et al.  A functional analysis of Pleistocene bone tools from two sites in Southwest Tasmania , 1990 .

[21]  L. F. Barrett,et al.  Handbook of Emotions , 1993 .

[22]  F. Neiman Stylistic Variation in Evolutionary Perspective: Inferences from Decorative Diversity and Interassemblage Distance in Illinois Woodland Ceramic Assemblages , 1995, American Antiquity.

[23]  Rhys Jones Tasmanian Archaeology: Establishing the Sequences , 1995 .

[24]  J. N. Smith,et al.  Dating and context of three middle stone age sites with bone points in the Upper Semliki Valley, Zaire. , 1995, Science.

[25]  J. Yellen,et al.  A middle stone age worked bone industry from Katanda, Upper Semliki Valley, Zaire. , 1995, Science.

[26]  D. Lancy Playing on the Mother-Ground: Cultural Routines for Children's Development. Culture and Human Development. , 1998 .

[27]  F. Berkes,et al.  Transmission of Indigenous Knowledge and Bush Skills Among the Western James Bay Cree Women of Subarctic Canada , 1997 .

[28]  Mark E. Madsen,et al.  Population Structure, Cultural Transmission, and Frequency Seriation , 1997 .

[29]  S. Frank Foundations of Social Evolution , 2019 .

[30]  K. Schlag Why Imitate, and If So, How?, : A Boundedly Rational Approach to Multi-armed Bandits , 1998 .

[31]  R. Boyd,et al.  The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. , 1998 .

[32]  Joseph Henrich,et al.  Cultural transmission and the diffusion of innovations : adoption dynamics indicate that biased cultural transmission is the predominate force in behavioral change and much of sociocultural evolution , 2001 .

[33]  Stephen Shennan,et al.  Cultural learning in hominids: a behavioural ecological approach , 1999 .

[34]  Ian J. McNiven,et al.  Australian coastal archaeology , 1999 .

[35]  W. Rivers Psychology and Ethnology , 1999 .

[36]  R. Selten,et al.  Bounded rationality: The adaptive toolbox , 2000 .

[37]  Marian Perkins,et al.  The Nurture Assumption: Why Children Turn Out the Way They Do , 2000, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[38]  R. Aunger The Life History of Culture Learning in a Face‐to‐Face Society , 2000 .

[39]  S. Shennan Demography and Cultural Innovation: a Model and its Implications for the Emergence of Modern Human Culture , 2001, Cambridge Archaeological Journal.

[40]  J. Henrich,et al.  The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. , 2001, Evolution and human behavior : official journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society.

[41]  Joseph Henrich,et al.  Decision-making, cultural transmission and adaptation in economic anthropology , 2001 .

[42]  R. Boyd,et al.  On Modeling Cognition and Culture: Why cultural evolution does not require replication of representations , 2002 .

[43]  B. Gott Fire‐Making in Tasmania: Absence of Evidence Is Not Evidence of Absence1 , 2002, Current Anthropology.

[44]  Jean Ensminger Theory in economic anthropology , 2002 .

[45]  Michelle Y. Merrill,et al.  Orangutan Cultures and the Evolution of Material Culture , 2003, Science.

[46]  M. Alvard,et al.  The adaptive nature of culture , 2003 .

[47]  S. Shennan Genes, Memes, and Human History: Darwinian Archaeology and Cultural Evolution , 2003 .

[48]  J. Henrich,et al.  The evolution of cultural evolution , 2003 .

[49]  E. Bassett Reconsidering Evidence of Tasmanian Fishing , 2004 .