Microsaccades are directed towards the midpoint between targets in a variably cued attention task

Reliable, non-invasive biomarkers that reveal the internal state of a subject are an invaluable tool for neurological diagnoses. Small fixational eye movements, called microsaccades, are a candidate biomarker thought to reflect a subject’s focus of attention (1, 2). The linkage between the direction of microsaccades and attention has mainly been demonstrated using explicit and unambiguous attentional cues. However, the natural world is seldom predictable and rarely provides unambiguous information. Thus, a useful biomarker must be robust to such changes in environmental statistics. To determine how well microsaccades reveal visual-spatial attention across behavioral contexts, we analyzed these fixational eye movements in monkeys performing a conventional change detection task. The task included two stimulus locations and variable cue validities across blocks of trials. Subjects were adept at the task, showing precise and graded modulations of visual attention for subtle target changes and performing better and faster when the cue was more reliable (3). However, over tens of thousands of microsaccades, we found no difference in microsaccade direction between cued locations when cue variability was high nor between hit and miss trials. Instead, microsaccades were made towards the midpoint of the two target locations, not towards individual targets. Our results suggest that the direction of microsaccades should be interpreted with caution and may not be a reliable measure of covert spatial attention in more complex viewing conditions. Significance Statement Small fixational eye movements called microsaccades are thought to “point” towards a location that is being attended in the visual periphery. This phenomenon has largely been studied using visual cues that unambiguously indicate the location of the upcoming stimulus change. Because the natural world is rarely unambiguous, we studied the relationship between microsaccade direction and spatial attention using less reliable cues. We found that monkeys’ microsaccade directions in a standard visuospatial attention task did not indicate the animals’ focus of attention, despite behavioral and neuronal evidence of spatial attention. Instead, microsaccades were made towards the midpoint between the target locations in both animals, suggesting a more complex relationship between microsaccades and attention in naturalistic settings.

[1]  Yi Yang,et al.  V1-bypassing suppression leads to direction-specific microsaccade modulation in visual coding and perception , 2022, Nature Communications.

[2]  W. Einhäuser,et al.  Pupillometry as an integrated readout of distinct attentional networks , 2022, Trends in Neurosciences.

[3]  Zhiguo Wang,et al.  The dynamics of microsaccade amplitude reflect shifting of covert attention , 2022, Consciousness and Cognition.

[4]  A. Nobre,et al.  Functional but not obligatory link between microsaccades and neural modulation by covert spatial attention , 2021, Nature Communications.

[5]  Leor N. Katz,et al.  Microsaccades as a marker not a cause for attention-related modulation , 2021, bioRxiv.

[6]  Daniel S. Kluger,et al.  Coupling of pupil- and neuronal population dynamics reveals diverse influences of arousal on cortical processing , 2021, bioRxiv.

[7]  S. Treue,et al.  Sustained spatial attention accounts for the direction bias of human microsaccades , 2020, Scientific Reports.

[8]  Sanjeev B. Khanna,et al.  The eyes reflect an internal cognitive state hidden in the population activity of cortical neurons , 2020, bioRxiv.

[9]  Francisco M. Costela,et al.  Microsaccade generation requires a foveal anchor , 2020, Journal of eye movement research.

[10]  Martina Poletti,et al.  Task-driven visual exploration at the foveal scale , 2019, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[11]  Robert Desimone,et al.  Enhanced Neural Processing by Covert Attention only during Microsaccades Directed toward the Attended Stimulus , 2018, Neuron.

[12]  Ziad M Hafed,et al.  Dynamics of fixational eye position and microsaccades during spatial cueing: the case of express microsaccades. , 2018, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  S. Martinez-Conde,et al.  Microsaccade Characteristics in Neurological and Ophthalmic Disease , 2018, Front. Neurol..

[14]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Graded Neuronal Modulations Related to Visual Spatial Attention , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  J. Victor,et al.  The unsteady eye: an information-processing stage, not a bug , 2015, Trends in Neurosciences.

[16]  P. Binda,et al.  Pupil size reflects the focus of feature-based attention. , 2014, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  Sharon M. Crook,et al.  Fixational Eye Movement Correction of Blink-Induced Gaze Position Errors , 2014, PloS one.

[18]  D. Heeger,et al.  Spontaneous Microsaccades Reflect Shifts in Covert Attention , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  On the Dissociation between Microsaccade Rate and Direction after Peripheral Cues: Microsaccadic Inhibition Revisited , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  Martina Poletti,et al.  Microscopic Eye Movements Compensate for Nonhomogeneous Vision within the Fovea , 2013, Current Biology.

[21]  S. Martinez-Conde,et al.  The impact of microsaccades on vision: towards a unified theory of saccadic function , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[22]  Xoana G. Troncoso,et al.  Microsaccadic Efficacy and Contribution to Foveal and Peripheral Vision , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  Microsaccadic Suppression of Visual Bursts in the Primate Superior Colliculus , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  J. Braun,et al.  Rare but precious: Microsaccades are highly informative about attentional allocation , 2010, Vision Research.

[25]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  When do microsaccades follow spatial attention? , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[26]  M. Carrasco,et al.  Attention trades off spatial acuity , 2009, Vision Research.

[27]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  A Neural Mechanism for Microsaccade Generation in the Primate Superior Colliculus , 2009, Science.

[28]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Toward a model of microsaccade generation: the case of microsaccadic inhibition. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[29]  Martina Poletti,et al.  Miniature eye movements enhance fine spatial detail , 2007, Nature.

[30]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Fixational Eye Movements Are Not an Index of Covert Attention , 2007, Psychological science.

[31]  Xoana G. Troncoso,et al.  Microsaccades Counteract Visual Fading during Fixation , 2005, Neuron.

[32]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Crossmodal coupling of oculomotor control and spatial attention in vision and audition , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention , 2003, Vision Research.

[34]  Geoffrey M. Ghose,et al.  Attentional modulation in visual cortex depends on task timing , 2002, Nature.

[35]  James J. Clark,et al.  Microsaccades as an overt measure of covert attention shifts , 2002, Vision Research.

[36]  George R. Mangun,et al.  Sustained visual-spatial attention produces costs and benefits in response time and evoked neural activity , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  R. London Eye Movement Basics for the Clinician , 1997 .

[38]  F A Miles,et al.  Release of fixation for pursuit and saccades in humans: evidence for shared inputs acting on different neural substrates. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  B. Richmond,et al.  Implantation of magnetic search coils for measurement of eye position: An improved method , 1980, Vision Research.

[40]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.

[41]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[42]  Martina Poletti,et al.  A compact field guide to the study of microsaccades: Challenges and functions , 2016, Vision Research.

[43]  S. Martinez-Conde Fixational eye movements in normal and pathological vision. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[44]  L. Stark,et al.  The main sequence, a tool for studying human eye movements , 1975 .