‘Beauty Is No Quality in Things Themselves’: Epistemic Motivation Affects Implicit Preferences for Art
暂无分享,去创建一个
Antonio Chirumbolo | Ambra Brizi | Stefano Mastandrea | L. Mannetti | A. Chirumbolo | Lucia Mannetti | S. Mastandrea | A. Brizi | Ambra Brizi
[1] Tom Pyszczynski,et al. Windows into nothingness: terror management, meaninglessness, and negative reactions to modern art. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[2] Larry E. Toothaker,et al. Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions , 1991 .
[3] Adrian Furnham,et al. Personality and judgements of abstract, pop art, and representational paintings , 2001 .
[4] S. Marković,et al. Automatic processes in aesthetic judgment: Insights from the Implicit Association Test , 2012 .
[5] J. de Houwer,et al. Implicit measures: A normative analysis and review. , 2009, Psychological bulletin.
[6] A. Furnham,et al. Personality and art preferences , 1988 .
[7] G. Cupchik,et al. Individual differences as predictors of preference in visual art , 1985 .
[8] M. Kossowska. Motivation towards closure and cognitive processes: An individual differences approach. , 2007 .
[9] M. Ferguson,et al. Beyond behaviorism: on the automaticity of higher mental processes. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.
[10] Thomas Jacobsen,et al. Beauty and the brain: culture, history and individual differences in aesthetic appreciation , 2010, Journal of anatomy.
[11] R. Nisbett,et al. Culture and Aesthetic Preference: Comparing the Attention to Context of East Asians and Americans , 2008, Personality & social psychology bulletin.
[12] A. Kruglanski,et al. On Leaping to Conclusions When Feeling Tired: Mental Fatigue Effects on Impressional Primacy , 1996 .
[13] E. Higgins,et al. Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. , 1996 .
[14] A. Kruglanski,et al. Motivated closing of the mind: "seizing" and "freezing". , 1996, Psychological review.
[15] Stefano Mastandrea,et al. Preferences for ancient and modern art museums: Visitor experiences and personality characteristics. , 2009 .
[16] Adrian Furnham,et al. Personality and preference for surreal paintings , 1997 .
[17] E. Orehek. Motivation Towards Closure and Cognitive Resources : An Individual Differences Approach , 2010 .
[18] P. Costa,et al. Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO-Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) , 1992 .
[19] F. Boselie,et al. Disjunctive Ambiguity as a Determinant of the Aesthetic Attractivity of Visual Patterns , 1994 .
[20] A. Kruglanski,et al. Effects of Epistemic Motivations on the Use of Accessible Constructs in Social Judgment , 1995 .
[21] D. Yves von Cramon,et al. Brain correlates of aesthetic judgment of beauty , 2006, NeuroImage.
[22] Alain Van Hiel,et al. Effects of Ambiguity and Need for Closure on the Acquisition of Information , 2002 .
[23] M. Zuckerman. Sensation Seeking : Beyond the Optimal Level of Arousal , 1979 .
[24] G. Carrus,et al. The Automatic Aesthetic Evaluation of Different Art and Architectural Styles , 2011 .
[25] Jens Förster,et al. How temporal distance changes novices' attitudes towards unconventional arts , 2008 .
[26] S. Mastandrea,et al. Implicit and Explicit Aesthetic Evaluation of Design Objects , 2014 .
[27] The Experimental Psychology of Beauty , 2015 .
[28] Brian A. Nosek,et al. Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[29] A. Kruglanski,et al. Cognitive and Social Consequences of the Need for Cognitive Closure , 1997 .
[30] H. Eysenck. THE GENERAL FACTOR IN AESTHETIC JUDGEMENTS1 , 1940 .
[31] Brian A. Nosek,et al. The Implicit Association Test at Age 7: A Methodological and Conceptual Review , 2007 .
[32] Serge Moscovici,et al. European Association of Experimental Social Psychology , 1967 .
[33] A. Kruglanski,et al. The freezing and unfreezing of lay-inferences: Effects on impressional primacy, ethnic stereotyping, and numerical anchoring ☆ , 1983 .
[34] A. Greenwald,et al. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[35] Gregory J. Feist,et al. Openness to Experience, Non-Conformity, and the Preference for Abstract Art , 2004 .
[36] Karen B. Schloss,et al. Visual aesthetics and human preference. , 2013, Annual review of psychology.
[37] N. Kettlewell,et al. The Effect of Subject Matter and Degree of Realism on Aesthetic Preferences for Paintings , 1990 .
[38] N. Schwarz,et al. Processing Fluency and Aesthetic Pleasure: Is Beauty in the Perceiver's Processing Experience? , 2004, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.
[39] D. Berlyne,et al. Aesthetics and Psychobiology , 1975 .
[40] Gerald Matthews,et al. Handbook of Individual Differences in Cognition , 2010 .
[41] Julia Mueller,et al. Can science account for taste? Psychological insights into art appreciation , 2011 .
[42] P. Silvia. Cognitive Appraisals and Interest in Visual Art: Exploring an Appraisal Theory of Aesthetic Emotions , 2005 .
[43] H. Eysenck. ‘TYPE’‐FACTORS IN AESTHETIC JUDGEMENTS , 1941 .
[44] A. Kruglanski. Motivated social cognition: Principles of the interface. , 1996 .
[45] Arie W. Kruglanski,et al. Motivations for judging and knowing: Implications for causal attribution. , 1990 .
[46] J. Bargh. Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. , 1989 .
[47] H. Leder,et al. A model of aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic judgments. , 2004, British journal of psychology.
[48] Lucy Gilson,et al. Curating collections of ideas: Museum as metaphor in the management of creativity☆ , 2013 .
[49] Marco Bertamini,et al. Implicit Association of Symmetry with Positive Valence, High Arousal and Simplicity , 2013 .
[50] Michael A. Olson,et al. Implicit measures in social cognition. research: their meaning and use. , 2003, Annual review of psychology.
[51] Shimul Melwani,et al. The Bias Against Creativity , 2012, Psychological science.
[52] A. Kruglanski,et al. Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[53] Helmut Leder,et al. Mastering style - Effects of explicit style-related information, art knowledge and affective state on appreciation of abstract paintings , 2006 .
[54] Marco Bertamini,et al. Implicit affective evaluation of visual symmetry. , 2012, Emotion.
[55] David Hume,et al. Essays, Moral, Political, And Literary , 2012 .
[56] William S. Griscom,et al. Accounting for taste: Individual differences in preference for harmony , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[57] A. Kruglanski,et al. Three decades of lay epistemics: The why, how, and who of knowledge formation , 2009 .
[58] Gerben A. van Kleef,et al. Who's afraid of red, yellow and blue? Need for cognitive closure predicts aesthetic preferences , 2012 .
[59] C. D. De Dreu,et al. Motivated Information Processing in Group Judgment and Decision Making , 2008, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.
[60] E. Higgins,et al. Handbook of motivation and cognition : foundations of social behavior , 1991 .
[61] Claudia Mello-Thoms,et al. Visual interest in pictorial art during an aesthetic experience. , 2007, Spatial vision.
[62] Adrian Furnham,et al. Personality and aesthetic preference in Spain and England: two studies relating sensation seeking and openness to experience to liking for paintings and music , 2000 .
[63] A. Kruglanski. The Psychology of Closed Mindedness , 2004 .